Skip to main content
Replication

Replication of Study 1 in “Differentiating Social and Personal Power” by Lammers, Stoker, and Stapel (2009)

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000388

Abstract. We performed an independent, direct, and better powered (N = 295) replication of Study 1, an experiment (N = 113) by Lammers, Stoker, and Stapel (2009). Lammers and colleagues distinguished between social power (influence over others) and personal power (freedom from the influence of others), and found support for their predictions that the two forms of power produce opposite effects on stereotyping, but parallel effects on behavioral approach. Our results did not replicate the effects on behavioral approach, but partially replicated the effects on stereotyping. Compared to personal power, social power produced less stereotyping, but neither form of power differed significantly from the control condition, and effect sizes were considerably lower than the original estimates. Potential explanations are discussed.

References

  • Anderson, C., & Galinsky, A. D. (2006). Power, optimism, and risk-taking. European Journal of Social Psychology, 36, 511–536. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.324 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Antonakis, J., Bendahan, S., Jacquart, P., & Lalive, R. (2010). On making causal claims: A review and recommendations. The Leadership Quarterly, 21, 1086–1120. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2010.10.010 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bendahan, S., Zehnder, C., Pralong, F. P., & Antonakis, J. (2015). Leader corruption depends on power and testosterone. The Leadership Quarterly, 26, 101–122. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2014.07.010 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Blader, S. L., & Chen, Y.-R. (2012). Differentiating the effects of status and power: A justice perspective. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102, 994–1014. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026651 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cesario, J. (2014). Priming, replication, and the hardest science. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 9, 40–48. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691613513470 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Chen, S., Ybarra, O., & Kiefer, A. K. (2004). Power and impression formation: The effects of power on the desire for morality and competence information. Social Cognition, 22, 391–421. https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.22.4.391.38296 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cislak, A., Cichocka, A., Wojcik, A. D., & Frankowska, N. (2018). Power corrupts, but control does not: What stands behind the effects of holding high positions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 44, 944–957. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167218757456 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Côté, S., Kraus, M. W., Cheng, B. H., Oveis, C., Löwe, I., Lian, H., & Keltner, D. (2011). Social power facilitates the effect of prosocial orientation on empathic accuracy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101, 217–232. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023171 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • DeCelles, K., DeRue, D. S., Margolis, J. D., & Ceranic, T. L. (2012). Does power corrupt or enable: When and why power facilitates self-interested behavior. Journal of Applied Psychology, 97, 681–689. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026811 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dépret, E. F., & Fiske, S. T. (1993). Social cognition and power: Some cognitive consequences of social structure as a source of control deprivation. In G. WearyF. GleicherK. MarshEds., Control motivation and social cognition (pp. 176–202). New York, NY: Springer. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Earp, B. D., & Trafimow, D. (2015). Replication, falsification, and the crisis of confidence in social psychology. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 621. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00621.R First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Emerson, R. M. (1962). Power dependence relations. American Sociological Review, 27, 31–41. https://doi.org/10.2307/2089716 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fiedler, K., & Prager, J. (2018). The regression trap and other pitfalls of replication science – illustrated by the report of the open science collaboration. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 40, 115–124. https://doi.org/10.1080/01973533.2017.1421953 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fiske, S. T. (1993). Controlling other people: The impact of power on stereotyping. American Psychologist, 48, 621–628. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.48.6.621 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fiske, S. T., & Dépret, E. (1996). Control, interdependence and power: Understanding social cognition in its social context. European Review of Social Psychology, 7, 31–61. https://doi.org/10.1080/14792779443000094 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • French, J. R. P. Jr., & Raven, B. (1959). The bases of social power. In D. CartwrightEd., Studies in social power (pp. 150–167). Oxford, UK: University of Michigan. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Galinsky, A. D., Gruenfeld, D. H., & Magee, J. C. (2003). From power to action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 453–466. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.3.453 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Galinsky, A. D., Magee, J. C., Gruenfeld, D. H., Whitson, J. A., & Liljenquist, K. A. (2008). Power reduces the press of the situation: Implications for creativity, conformity, and dissonance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 1450–1466. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0012633 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Galinsky, A. D., Magee, J. C., Inesi, M. E., & Gruenfeld, D. H. (2006). Power and perspectives not taken. Psychological Science, 17, 1068–1074. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01824.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Goodwin, S. A., Gubin, A., Fiske, S. T., & Yzerbyt, V. Y. (2000). Power can bias impression processes: Stereotyping subordinates by default and by design. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 3, 227–256. https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430200003003001 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gruenfeld, D. H., Inesi, M. E., Magee, J. C., & Galinsky, A. D. (2008). Power and the objectification of social targets. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 111–127. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.95.1.111 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Guinote, A., & Phillips, A. (2010). Power can increase stereotyping: Evidence from managers and subordinates in the hotel industry. Social Psychology, 41, 3–9. https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000002 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Handgraaf, M. J. J., Van Dijk, E., Vermunt, R. C., Wilke, H. A. M., & De Dreu, C. K. W. (2008). Less power or powerless? Egocentric empathy caps and the irony of having little versus no power in social decision making. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 1136–1149. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.95.5.1136 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hofstede, G. (1983). National cultures in four dimensions. International Studies of Management & Organization, 13, 46–74. https://doi.org/10.1080/00208825.1983.11656358 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Inesi, M. E., Lee, S. Y., & Rios, K. (2014). Objects of desire: Subordinate ingratiation triggers self-objectification among powerful. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 53, 19–30. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2014.01.010 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kahneman, D. (2012). Kahneman’s open letter from 26 September. Retrieved from http://www.nature.com/polopoly_fs/7.6716.1349271308!/suppinfoFile/Kahneman%20Letter.pdf First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Keltner, D., Gruenfeld, D. H., & Anderson, C. (2003). Power, approach, and inhibition. Psychological Review, 110, 265–284. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.110.2.265 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kipnis, D. (1972). Does power corrupt? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 24, 33–41. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0033390 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kipnis, D., Castell, P. J., Gergen, M., & Mauch, D. (1976). Metamorphic effects of power. Journal of Applied Psychology, 61, 127–135. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.61.2.127 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lammers, J., Galinsky, A. D., Gordijn, E. H., & Otten, S. (2008). Illegitimacy moderates the effects of power on approach. Psychological Science, 19, 558–564. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02123.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lammers, J., Stapel, D. A., & Galinsky, A. D. (2010). Power increases hypocrisy: Moralizing in reasoning, immorality in behavior. Psychological Science, 21, 737–744. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610368810 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lammers, J., Stoker, J. I., & Stapel, D. A. (2009). Differentiating social and personal power: Opposite effects on stereotyping, but parallel effects on behavioral approach tendencies. Psychological Science, 20, 1543–1548. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02479.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lammers, J., Stoker, J. I., & Stapel, D. A. (2010). Power and behavioral approach orientation in existing power relations and the mediating effect of income. European Journal of Social Psychology, 40, 543–551. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.702 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Maner, J. K., Kaschak, M. P., & Jones, J. L. (2010). Social power and the advent of action. Social Cognition, 28, 122–132. https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.2010.28.1.122 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mast, M. S., Jonas, K., & Hall, J. A. (2009). Give a person power and he or she will show interpersonal sensitivity: The phenomenon and its why and when. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 835–850. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016234 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Newell, B. R., & Shanks, D. R. (2014). Unconscious influences on decision making: A critical review. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 37, 1–18. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X12003214 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Overbeck, J. R., & Park, B. (2001). When power does not corrupt: Superior individuation processes among powerful perceivers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 549–565. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.81.4.549 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Overbeck, J. R., & Park, B. (2006). Powerful perceivers, powerless objects: Flexibility of powerholders' social attention. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 99, 227–243. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2005.10.003 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pratto, F. (2016). On power and empowerment. British Journal of Social Psychology, 55, 1–20. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjso.12135 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ranehill, E., Dreber, A., Johannesson, M., Leiberg, S., Sul, S., & Weber, R. A. (2015). Assessing the robustness of power posing: No effect on hormones and risk tolerance in a large sample of men and women. Psychological Science, 26, 653–656. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614553946 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schaerer, M., du Plessis, C., Yap, A. J., & Thau, S. (2018). Low power individuals in social power research: A quantitative review, theoretical framework, and empirical test. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 149, 73–96. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2018.08.004 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Simons, D. J. (2014). The value of direct replication. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 9, 76–80. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691613514755 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Simonsohn, U. (2015). Small telescopes: Detectability and the evaluation of replication results. Psychological Science, 26, 559–569. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614567341 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Smith, P. K., & Bargh, J. A. (2008). Nonconscious effects of power on basic approach and avoidance tendencies. Social Cognition, 26, 1–24. https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.2008.26.1.1 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sturm, R. E., & Antonakis, J. (2015). Interpersonal power: A review, critique, and research agenda. Journal of Management, 41, 136–163. https://doi.org/10.1177/0149206314555769 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Unkelbach, C. (2016). Increasing replicability. Social Psychology, 47, 1–3. https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000270 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Van Dijke, M., & Poppe, M. (2006). Striving for personal power as a basis for social power dynamics. European Journal of Social Psychology, 36, 537–556. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.351 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Zhang, M., & Smith, P. K. (2018). Does power reduce temporal discounting? Commentary on Joshi and Fast (2013). Psychological Science, 29, 1010–1019. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797617754219 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar