Skip to main content
Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1026//0033-3042.31.3.116

Zusammenfassung. Das Ziel der vorliegenden Studie war es, die Rolle metakognitiver Überwachungsprozesse in dem angewandten Kontext einer Identifikationsaufgabe zu überprüfen. Dazu wurde Vorschülern, Zweitkläßlern und Erwachsenen ein kurzer Videofilm über einen Konflikt zwischen Kindern vorgeführt. Drei Wochen später wurden der Hälfte der Versuchspersonen irreführende Distraktoren vorgelegt. Vier Wochen nach der Videodemonstration wurde die Identifikationsaufgabe, bei der die sieben Gesichter der beteiligten Kinder aus dem Film unter Distraktoren wiederzuerkennen waren, in zwei verschiedenen Versionen (sequentielle und simultane Darbietung der Photos) durchgeführt. Zur Erfassung metakognitiver Kompetenzen wurden Sicherheitsurteile zu den Identifikationen erhoben. Die Auswertung der Wiedererkennungsleistungen ergab insgesamt keinen klaren Alterstrend: bessere Identifikationsraten der Erwachsenen im Vergleich zu den Kindern ließen sich lediglich bei sequentieller, nicht aber bei simultaner Darbietung der Photos finden. Dabei waren in allen Altersgruppen negative Effekte der vorangegangenen Irreführung nachweisbar. In bezug auf die Sicherheitsurteile konnte festgestellt werden, daß sich die Kinder sowohl bei korrekten als auch bei falschen Identifikationen sicherer waren als die Erwachsenen und somit Schwierigkeiten hatten, die Sicherheit ihrer Erinnerung adäqaut einzuschätzen. Damit konnten Befunde aus der Grundlagenforschung zum prozeduralen Metagedächtnis bestätigt und deren Relevanz in einem angewandten Kontext aufgezeigt werden.


The influence of metacognitions and prior misinformations on children's eyewitness identifications

Abstract. The present study was conducted to examine the role of metacognitive monitoring processes in the applied setting of an eyewitness identification task. Preschoolers, second graders, and adults were shown a short video about a conflict between children. Three weeks later, half of the subjects were misled with photos showing distractor faces. Four weeks after watching the video, all subjects were given the identification task with the seven target faces from the video and fourteen distractor items, seven of which had been used in the misleading phase a week earlier. The photos were either presented one after the other or in an array of three photos with the target present in the line-up. For each photo, confidence judgments were gathered. The results did not reveal a clear age trend. In the sequential presentation condition, older children and adults outperformed preschoolers, whereas in the simultaneous presentation of the photos no age trend emerged. However, in all age groups a negative effect of prior misinformation was found. Confidence judgments of children proved to be overoptimistic compared to those of adults with regard to both correct and incorrect identifications. Thus, findings from laboratory studies on the development of metamemory were confirmed and their relevance for an applied setting was shown.

Literatur

  • Baker, L. , Brown, A. L. (1984). Metacognitive skills and reading. In P. D. Pearson, M. Kamil, R. Barr & P. Mosenthal (Eds.), Handbook of reading research (pp. 353-394). New York: Longman . First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Beal, C. R. , Schmitt, K. L. , Dekle, D. J. (1995). Eyewitness identification of children: Effects of absolute judgments, nonverbal response options, and event encoding. Law and Human Behavior , 19, 197– 217 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bruce, V. , Young, A. (1986). Understanding face recognition. British Journal of Psychology , 77, 305– 327 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bruck, M. , Ceci, S. J. (1997). The description of children's suggestibility. In N. L. Stern, P. A. Ornstein, B. Tversky & C. Brainerd (Eds.), Memory for everyday and emotional events (pp. 371-400). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum . First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Carey, S. (1981). The development of face perception. In G. Davies, H. Ellis & J. Sheperd (Eds.), Perceiving and remembering faces (pp. 9-34). London: Academic Press . First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Carey, S. (1996). Perceptual classification and expertise. In R. Gelman & K.-F. An (Eds.), Perceptual and cognitive development (pp. 49-69). San Diego: Academic Press . First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Cassel, W. S. , Roebers, C. M. , Bjorklund, D. F. (1996). Developmental patterns of eyewitness responses to repeated and increasingly suggestive questions. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology , 61, 116– 133 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ceci, S. J. , Bruck, M. (1993). Suggestibility of the child witness: A historical review and synthesis. Psychological Bulletin , 113, 3 403– 439 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ceci, S. J. , Bruck, M. (1998). Children's testimony: Applied and basic issues. In W. Damon, D. Kuhn & R. S. Siegler (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology. Cognition, perception, and language (Vol. 2, pp. 713-774). New York: Wiley . First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Davies, G. M. , Stevenson-Robb, Y. , Flin, R. (1988). Tales out of school: Children's memory for an unexpected incident. In M. Gruneberg, P. Morris & R. Sykes (Eds.), Practical aspects of memory (Vol. 1, pp. 122-127). Chichester: Wiley . First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Davies, G. , Tarrant, A. , Flin, R. (1989). Close encounters of the witness kind: Children's memory for a simulated health inspection. British Journal of Psychology , 80, 415– 429 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Diamond, R. , Carey, S. (1977). Developmental changes in the representation of faces. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology , 23, 1– 22 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dufresne, A. , Kobasigawa, A. (1989). Children's spontaneous allocation of study time: Differential and sufficient aspects. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology , 47, 274– 296 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Flavell, J. H. (1970). Developmental studies of mediated memory. In H. W. Reese & L. P. Lipsitt (Eds.), Advances in child development and behavior (pp. 181-211). New York: Academic Press . First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Flin, R. , Boon, J. , Knox, A. , Bull, R. (1992). The effect of a five-month delay on children's and adults' eyewitness memory. British Journal of Psychology , 83, 323– 336 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Goodman, G. S. , Bottoms, B. , Schwartz-Kenney, B. M. , Rudy, L. (1991). Children's memory for a stressful event: Improving children's reports. Journal of Narrative and Life History , 1, 69– 99 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Goodman, G. S. , Hirschman, J. E. , Hepps, D. , Rudy, L. (1991). Children's memory for stressful events. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly , 37, 109– 149 . First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Goodman, G. S. , Reed, R. S. (1986). Age differences in eyewitness testimony. Law and Human Behavior , 10, 4 317– 332 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Grier, J. B. (1971). Nonparametric indexes for sensitivity and bias: Computing formulas. Psychological Bulletin , 75, 424– 429 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gross, J. , Hayne, H. (1996). Eyewitness identification by 5- to 6-year-old children. Law and Human Behavior , 20, 359– 373 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Körkel, J. , Schneider, W. (1991). Domain-specific versus metacognitive knowledge effects on text recall and comprehension. In M. Carretero, M. Pope, R. J. Simons & J. I. Pozo (Eds.), Learning and instruction: European research in an international context (Vol. 3, pp. 311-323). Oxford: Pergamon Press . First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Lindsay, R. C. L. , Wells, G. L. (1985). Improving eyewitness identifications from lineups: Simultaneous versus sequential lineup presentation. Journal of Applied Psychology , 70, 556– 564 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Marin, B. V. , Holmes, D. L. , Guth, M. , Kovac, P. (1979). The potential of children as eyewitnesses. Law and Human Behavior , 3, 4 295– 306 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mazzoni, G. , Cornoldi, C. , Tomat, L. , Vecchi, T. (1997). Remembering the grocery shopping list: A study on metacognitive biases. Applied Cognitive Psychology , 11, 253– 267 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Parker, J. F. , Carranza, L. E. (1989). Eyewitness testimony of children in target-present and target-absent lineups. Law and Human Behavior , 13, 133– 149 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Parker, J. F. , Ryan, V. (1993). An attempt to reduce guessing behavior in children's and adults' eyewitness identifications. Law and Human Behavior , 17, 11– 26 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pressley, M. , Levin, J. R. , Ghatala, E. S. , Ahmad, M. (1987). Test monitoring in young grade school children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology , 43, 96– 111 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Roebers, C. , Rieber, F. , Schneider, W. (1995). Zeugenaussagen und Suggestibilität als Funktion der Erinnerungsgenauigkeit: Eine entwicklungspsychologische Studie. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie , 27, 210– 225 . First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Schneider, W. (1989). Zur Entwicklung des Meta-Gedächtnisses bei Kindern . Bern: Huber . First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Schneider, W. , Bjorklund, D. F. (1998). Memory. In W. Damon, D. Kuhn & R. S. Siegler (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Cognition, perception, and language (Vol. 2, pp. 467-521). New York: Wiley . First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Schneider, W. , Borkowski, J. G. , Kurtz, B. E. , Kerwin, K. (1986). Metamemory and motivation - A comparison of strategy use and performance in German and American children. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology , 17, 315– 336 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schneider, W. , Pressley, M. (1997). Memory development between 2 and 20 . Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum . First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Schwarzer, G. (1997). Kategorisierung von Gesichtern bei Kindern und Erwachsenen: Die Rolle konzeptuellen Wissens. Sprache und Kognition , 1, 14– 30 . First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Soppe, H. J. G. (1986). Children's recognition of unfamiliar faces: Developments and determinants. International Journal of Behavioral Development , 9, 219– 233 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sporer, S. L. (1993). Eyewitness identification accuracy, confidence, and decision times in simultaneous and sequential lineups. Journal of Applied Psychology , 78, 22– 33 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Stipek, D. J. , Roberts, T. A. , Sanborn, M. E. (1984). Preschool-age children's performance expectations for themselves and another child as a function of the incentive value of success and the salience of past performance. Child Development , 55, 1983– 1989 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Visé, M. (1998). Metagedächtnis, Vorwissen und textbezogenes Lernen: Zur Entwicklung der kurz- und langfristigen Gedächtnisleistung bei Schulkindern . Lengerich: Pabst . First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Weber, A. (1993). Autobiographische Erinnerung und Persönlichkeit . Frankfurt a. M.: Peter Lang . First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Wellman, H. M. (1977). Tip of the tongue and feeling of knowing experiences: A developmental study of memory monitoring. Child Development , 48, 13– 21 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar