Skip to main content
Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1026/0049-8637/a000097

In der vorliegenden Studie wurden die Handgeschicklichkeit, exekutive Funktionen und Schulleistungen bei Vorschulkindern mit und ohne Problemen in der Handgeschicklichkeit über einen 3-Jahres-Zeitraum untersucht. Insgesamt wurden N = 94 Kinder, die zu Beginn 5- oder 6-jährig waren, untersucht, davon 47 Risikokinder mit sehr niedrigen Leistungen in der Handgeschicklichkeit und 47 Vergleichskinder. Diese wurden anhand der Variablen Alter, sozioökonomischer Status und nonverbale Intelligenz bestimmt. Die Risikokinder zeigten persistierende Defizite in der Handkoordination über den gesamten Untersuchungszeitraum hinweg. Obwohl sich alle Kinder insgesamt deutlich in den exekutiven Funktionen verbesserten, zeigten die Risikokinder ferner eine bedeutsam schlechtere Interferenzkontrolle und kognitive Flexibilität als die Vergleichskinder. Auch die schulischen Leistungen in den Bereichen Lesen, Schreiben und Mathematik zu Beginn der Beschulung waren bei den Risikokindern niedriger als bei den Vergleichskindern. Diese Befunde deuten auf domänübergreifende Probleme bei inhibitorischen und/oder Automatisierungsprozessen bei Kindern mit Auffälligkeiten in der Handgeschicklichkeit hin und geben wichtige Hinweise auf notwendige Interventionsmaßnahmen.


Development of manual dexterity, executive functions, and scholastic achievement in children at risk for developmental coordination disorders

The present 3-year-longitudinal study investigated manual dexterity, executive functions, and scholastic achievement in kindergarten children with or without deficits in manual dexterity. The sample consisted of N = 94 children: 47 children with very low manual dexterity performance and 47 control children who were matched in age, socioeconomic status, and nonverbal intelligence. Children were 5 or 6 years old at the beginning of the study. The children at risk showed persistent deficits in manual dexterity, as well as consistently lower interference control and shifting performance. Both groups showed marked developmental progression in executive functions during the 3-year period. After transition to school, the children at risk showed lower reading, writing, and mathematics skills. The results suggest domain-general problems with inhibition and/or automatization processes in children with manual dexterity problems. Implications for interventions are discussed.

Literatur

  • Alloway, T. P. (2007). Working memory, reading, and mathematical skills in children with developmental coordination disorder. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 96, 20 – 36. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Alloway, T. P. & Archibald, L. (2008). Working memory and learning in children with developmental coordination disorder and specific language impairment. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 41, 251 – 262. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Alloway, T. P. , Gathercole, S. E. , Adams, A.-M. , Willis, C. , Eagle, R. & Lamont, E. (2005). Working memory and phonological awareness as predictors of progress towards early learning goals at school entry. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 23, 417 – 426. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Alloway, T. P. & Temple, K. J. (2007). A comparison of working memory skills and learning in children with developmental coordination disorder and moderate learning difficulties. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 21, 473 – 487. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Archibald, S. J. & Kerns, K. A. (1999). Identification and description of new tests of executive functioning in children. Child Neuropsychology, 5, 115 – 129. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Biederman, J. , Monuteaux, M. C. , Doyle, A. E. , Seidman, L. J. , Wilens, T. E. , Ferrero, F. Morgan , C. L. & Faraone, S. V. (2004). Impact of executive function deficits and Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) on academic outcomes in children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 72, 757 – 766. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bjorklund, D. F. & Harnishfeger, K. K. (2000). The evolution of inhibition mechanisms and their role in human cognition and behavior. In F. N. Dempster & C. J. Brainerd (Eds.), Interference and inhibition in cognition (pp. 141 – 173). New York: Academic Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Blair, C. & Razza, R. P. (2007). Relating effortful control, executive function, and false belief understanding to emerging math and literacy ability in kindergarten. Child Development, 78,647 – 663. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bös, K. (1987). Handbuch sportmotorischer Tests. Göttingen: Hogrefe. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Brocki K. C. & Bohlin, G. (2004). Executive functions in children aged 6 to 13: A dimensional and developmental study. Developmental Neuropsychology, 26, 571 – 593. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bull, R. , Espy, K. A. & Wiebe, S. A. (2008). Short-term memory, working memory, and executive functioning in preschoolers: Longitudinal predictors of mathematical achievement at age 7 years. Developmental Neuropsychology, 33, 205 – 228. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bull, R. & Scerif, G. (2001). Executive functioning as a predictor of children’s mathematics ability: Inhibition, switching, and working memory. Developmental Neuropsychology, 19, 273 – 293. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cantell, M. H. , Smyth, M. M. & Ahonen, T. P. (1994). Clumsiness in adolescence: educational, motor, and social outcomes of motor delay detected at 5 years. Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly, 11, 115 – 129. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cattell, R. B. , Weiss, R. H. & Osterland, J. (1997). CFT 1: Grundintelligenztest Skala 1 (5. Aufl.). Göttingen: Hogrefe. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Chambers, M. E. , Sugden, D. A. & Sinani, C. (2005). The nature of children with developmental coordination disorder. In D. A. Sugden & M. E. Chambers (Eds.), Children with developmental coordination disorder (pp 1 – 18). London: Whurr Publishers. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Dewey, D. , Kaplan, B. J. , Crawford, S. G. & Wilson, B. N. (2002). Developmental coordination disorder: Associated problems in attention, learning, and psychosocial adjustment. Human Movement Science, 21, 905 – 918. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Diamond, A. (2000). Close interrelation of motor development and cognitive development and of the cerebellum and prefrontal cortex. Child Development, 71, 44 – 56. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Diamond, A. (2002). Normal development of prefrontal cortex from birth to young adulthood: Cognitive functions, anatomy, and biochemistry. In D. Stuss & R. Knight (Eds.), Principles of frontal lobe function (pp. 466 – 503). New York: Oxford University Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Diamond, A. , Barnett, W. S. , Thomas, J. & Munro, S. (2007). Preschool program improves cognitive control. Science, 317, 1387 – 1388. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Duncan, G. J. , Claessens, A. , Huston, A. C. , Pagani, L. S. , Engel, M. , Sexton, H. , Dowsett, C. J. , Magnuson, K. , Klebanov, P. , Feinstein, L. , Brooks-Gunn, J. & Duckworth K. (2007). School readiness and later achievement. Developmental Psychology, 43, 1426 – 1446. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gathercole, S. E. , Alloway, T. P. , Willis, C. & Adams, A. M. (2006). Working memory in children with reading disabilities. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 93, 265 – 281. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Geary, D. C. & Hoard, M. K. (2001). Numerical and arithmetical deficits in learning-disabled children: Relation to dyscalculia and dyslexia. Aphasiology, 15, 635 – 647. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Geuze, R. H. Jongmans , M. J., Schoemaker , M. M. & Smits-Engelsman, B. C. M. (2001). Clinical and research diagnostic criteria for developmental coordination disorder: A review and discussion. Human Movement Science, 20, 7 – 47. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Grube, D. (2005). Entwicklung des Rechnens im Grundschulalter. In M. Hasselhorn, H. Marx & W. Schneider (Hrsg.), Diagnostik von Mathematikleistungen (S. 105 – 124). Göttingen: Hogrefe. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Haffner, J. , Baro, K. , Parzer, P. & Resch, F. (2005). HRT 1 – 4. Heidelberger Rechentest. Göttingen: Hogrefe. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Helmke, A. , Schneider, W. & Weinert, F. E. (1986). Quality of instruction and classroom learning outcomes: The German contribution to the IEA classroom environment study. Teaching and Teacher Education, 2, 1 – 18. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Henderson, S. E. , Sugden, D. A. & Barnett, A. L. (2007). M-ABC-2: Movement assessment Battery for Children –2. London: Harcourt. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Hill, E. L. (2001). Non-specific nature of specific language impairment: A review of the literature with regard to concomitant motor impairments. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 36, 149 – 171. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hughes, C. & Graham, A. (2002). Measuring executive functions in childhood: Problems and solutions? Child and Adolescent Mental Health, 7, 131 – 142. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Huizinga, M. , Dolan, C. & van der Molen, M. (2006). Age-related change in executive function: Developmental trends and a latent variable analysis. Neuropsychologia, 44, 2017 – 2036. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kaplan, B. J. , Wilson, B. N. , Dewey, D. & Crawford, S. G. (1998). DCD may not be a discrete disorder. Human Movement Science, 17, 471 – 490. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kastner, J. & Petermann, F. (2009). Entwicklungsbedingte Koordinationsstörung. Psychologische Rundschau, 60, 73 – 81. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Klicpera, C. , Gasteiger-Klicpera, B. & Schabmann, A. (1993). Lesen und Schreiben–Entwicklung und Schwierigkeiten. Die Wiener Längsschnittuntersuchungen über die Entwicklung, den Verlauf und die Ursachen von Lese- und Schreibschwierigkeiten in der Pflichtschulzeit. Bern: Huber. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Küspert, P. & Schneider, W. (1998). Würzburger Leise Leseprobe (WLLP). Göttingen: Hogrefe. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Lee, K. , Swee-Fong, N. , Ee-Lynn, N. & Zee-Ying, L. (2004). Working memory and literacy as predictors of performance on algebraic word problems. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 89, 140 – 158. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Livesey, D. , Keen, J. , Rouse, J. & White, F. (2006). The relationship between measures of executive function, motor performance and externalising behaviour in 5- and 6-year-old children. Human Movement Science, 25, 50 – 64. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mandich, A. , Buckholz, E. & Polatajko, H. (2002). On the ability of children with developmental coordination disorder (DCD) to inhibit response initiation: The Simon effect. Brain and Cognition, 50, 150 – 162. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mandich, A , Buckholz, E. & Polatajko, H. (2003). Children with developmental coordination disorder (DCD) and their inability to disengage ongoing attentional focus: More on inhibitory function. Brain and Cognition, 51, 346 – 356. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Manly, T. , Anderson, V. , Nimmo-Smith, I. , Turner, A. , Watson, P. & Robertson, H. (2001). The differential assessment of children’s attention: The test of everyday attention for children (TEA-Ch), normative sample and ADHD performance. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44, 1065 – 1081. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mayringer, H. & Wimmer, H. (2003). SLS 1 – 4. Salzburger Lese-Screening für die Klassenstufen 1 – 4. Bern: Huber. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • May, P. (2002). HSP 1 – 9. Hamburger Schreib-Probe 1 – 9. Göttingen: Hogrefe. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Michel, E. , Kauer, M. & Roebers, C. M. (2011). Motorische Koordinationsdefizite im Kindesalter: Welche Bedeutung haben kognitive Basisfunktionen? Kindheit und Entwicklung, 20, 49 – 59. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Michel, E. & Roebers, C. M. (2008). Children in regular and in special-needs classes: Cross-sectional and longitudinal comparisons. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 67, 249 – 259. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Michel, E. , Röthlisberger, M. , Neuenschwander, R. & Roebers, C. M. (2011). Development of cognitive skills in children with motor coordination impairments at 12 month follow-up. Child Neuropsychology, 17, 151 – 172. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Miyake, A. , Friedman, N. P. , Emerson, M. J. , Witzki, A. H. , Howerter, A. & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex ‘‘frontal lobe’’ tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49 – 100. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Norman, D. A. & Shallice, T. (1986). Attention to action: Willed and automatic control of behavior. In R. J. Davidson, G. E. Schwartz & D. Shapiro (Eds.), Consciousness and self-regulation: Advances in research and theory (Vol. 4, pp. 1 – 18). New York: Plenum. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Olivier, I. , Cuisinier, R. , Vaugoyeau, M. , Nougier, V. & Assaiante, C. (2007). Dual-task study of cognitive and postural interference in 7-year-olds and adults. Neuroreport, 18, 817 – 821. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Parker, H. & Larkin, D. (2003). Children’s co-ordination and developmental movement difficulty. In G. Savelsbergh, K. Davids , J. van der Kamp & S. J. Benett (Eds.), Development of movement co-ordination in children (pp. 107 – 132). London: Routledge First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Pennequin, V. , Sorel, O. & Fontaine, R. (2010). Motor planning between 4 and 7 years of age: Changes linked to executive functions. Brain and Cognition, 74, 107 – 111. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pennington, B. F. & Ozonoff, S. (1996). Executive functions and developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37, 51 – 87. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Piek, J. P. , Dyck, M. J. , Nieman, A. , Anderson, M. , Hay, D. , Smith, L. M. et al. (2004). The relationship between motor coordination, executive functioning and attention in school aged children. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 19, 1063 – 1076. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Posner, M. I. & Rothbart, M. K. (2007). Educating the human brain. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Querne, L. , Berquin, P. , Vernier-Hauvette, M.-P. , Fall, S. , Deltour, L. , Meyer, M.-E. et al. (2008). Dysfunction of the attentional brain network in children with developmental coordination disorder: A fMRI study. Brain Research, 1244, 89 – 102. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Remschmidt, H. , Schmidt, M.H. & Poustka, F. (2006). Multiaxiales Klassifikationsschema für psychische Störungen des Kindes- und Jugendalters nach ICD-10 der WHO. Bern: Huber. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Roebers, C. M. & Kauer, M. (2009). Motor and cognitive control in a normative sample of 7-year-olds. Developmental Science, 12, 175 – 181. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Roebers, C. M. , Röthlisberger, M. , Cimeli, P. , Michel, E. & Neuenschwander, R. (2011). School enrollment and executive functioning: A longitudinal perspective on developmental changes, the influence of learning context, and the prediction of pre-academic skills. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 8, 526 – 540 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Röthlisberger, M. , Neuenschwander, R. , Cimeli, P. , Michel, E. & Roebers, C. M. (2012). Improving executive functions in 5- and 6-year-olds: Evaluation of a small group intervention in prekindergarten and kindergarten children. Infant and Child Development, 21, 411 – 429. doi: 10.1002/icd.752. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Röthlisberger, M. , Neuenschwander, R. , Michel, E. & Roebers, C. M. (2010). Exekutive Funktionen: Zugrundeliegende kognitive Prozesse und deren Korrelate bei Kindern im späten Vorschulalter. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 42, 99 – 110. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Schmid, C. , Zoelch, C. & Roebers, C. M. (2008). Das Arbeitsgedächtnis von 4- bis 5-jährigen Kindern: Theoretische und empirische Analyse seiner Funktionen. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 40, 2 – 12. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Schott, N. & Roncesvalles, N. (2004) Motorische Ungeschicklichkeit. Zeitschrift für Sportpsychologie, 11, 147 – 162. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Sergeant, J. (2000). The cognitive-energetic model: An empirical approach to attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 24, 7 – 12. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Van der Sluis, S. , de Jong, P. F. & van der Leij, A. (2007). Executive functioning in children, and its relations with reasoning, reading, and arithmetic. Intelligence, 35, 427 – 449. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Visser, J. (2003). Developmental coordination disorder: A review of research on subtypes and comorbidities. Human Movement Science, 22, 479 – 493. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wassenberg, R. , Feron, F. , Kessels, A. , Hendriksen, J. , Kalff, A. , Kroes, M. , Hurks, P. , Beeren, M. , Jolles, J. & Vles, J. (2005). Relation between cognitive and motor performance in 5- to 6-year-old children: Results from a large-scale cross-sectional study. Child Development, 76, 1092 – 1103. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wegener, B. (1988). Kritik des Prestiges. Opladen: Westdeutscher Verlag. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Welsh, M. C. (2002). Development and clinical variations in executive functions. In D. L. Molfese & V. J. Molfese (Eds.), Developmental variations in learning (pp. 139 – 185). London: Routledge. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Woollacott, M. & Shumway-Cook, A. (2002). Attention and the control of posture and gait: A review of an emerging area of research. Gait Posture, 16, 1 – 14. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Zimmermann, P. , Gondan, M. & Fimm, B. (2002). KITAP Testbatterie zur Aufmerksamkeitsprüfung bei Kindern. Herzogenrath: Vera Fimm, Psychologische Testsysteme. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Zoelch, C. , Seitz, K. & Schumann-Hengsteler, R. (2005). From rag(bag)s to riches: measuring the developing central executive. In W. Schneider, R. Schumann-Hengsteler & B. Sodian (Eds.), Young children’s cognitive development. Interrelationships among executive functioning, working memory, verbal ability and theory of mind (pp. 39 – 69). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar