Skip to main content
Opinion

Theory of Mind Across the Lifespan?

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/2151-2604/a000330
Free first page

References

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Jolliffe, T., Mortimore, C. & Robertson, M. (1997). Another advanced test of theory of mind: Evidence from very high functioning adults with autism or Asperger Syndrome. Child Psychology & Psychiatry & Allied Disciplines, 38, 813–822. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01599.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Brink, K. A., Gray, K. & Wellman, H. M. (2017). Creepiness creeps in: Uncanny valley feelings are acquired in childhood. Child Development. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12999 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Brink, K. A. & Wellman, H. M. (in press). Technology as teacher: How children learn from social robots. In S. GrimmEd., Varieties of understanding: New perspectives from philosophy, psychology and theology, New York, NY: Oxford University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Carpendale, J. I. & Chandler, M. J. (1996). On the distinction between false belief understanding and subscribing to an interpretive theory of mind. Child Development, 67, 1686–1706. https://doi.org/10.2307/1131725 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Derksen, D. G., Hunsche, M. C., Giroux, M. E., Connolly, D. A. & Bernstein, D. M. (2018). A systematic review of theory of mind’s precursors and functions. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 226, 87–97. https://doi.org/10.1027/2151-2604/a000325 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Devine, R. T. & Hughes, C. (2013). Silent films and strange stories: Theory of mind, gender, and social experiences in middle childhood. Child Development, 84, 989–1003. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12017 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Happé, F. G. E. (1994). An advanced test of theory of mind: Understanding of story characters’ thoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped, and normal children and adults. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 129–154. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02172093 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lagattuta, K. M., Tashjian, S. M. & Kramer, H. J. (2018). Does the past shape anticipation for the future? Contributions of age and executive function to advanced theory of mind. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 226, 122–133. https://doi.org/10.1027/2151-2604/a000328 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Lane, J. D., Evans, E. M., Brink, K. A. & Wellman, H. M. (2016). Developing concepts of ordinary and extraordinary communication. Developmental Psychology, 52, 19–30. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000061 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lane, J. D., Wellman, H. M. & Evans, E. M. (2014). Approaching an understanding of omniscience from the preschool years to early adulthood. Developmental Psychology, 50, 2380–2392. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037715 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Marchetti, A., Manzi, F., Itakura, S. & Massaro, D. (2018). Theory of mind and humanoid robots from a lifespan perspective. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 226, 98–109. https://doi.org/10.1027/2151-2604/a000326 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Osterhaus, C., Koerber, S. & Sodian, B. (2016). Scaling of advanced theory‐of‐mind tasks. Child Development, 87, 1971–1991. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12566 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Perner, J. & Wimmer, H. (1985). “John thinks that Mary thinks that …”: Attribution of second-order beliefs by 5- to 10-year-old children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 39, 437–471. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-0965(85)90051-7 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rafetseder, E. & Perner, J. (2018). Belief and counterfactuality: A teleological theory of belief attribution. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 226, 110–121. https://doi.org/10.1027/2151-2604/a000327 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Richert, R. & Harris, P. L. (2006). The ghost in my body: Children’s developing concept of the soul. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 6, 409–427. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wellman, H. M., Fang, F. & Peterson, C. C. (2011). Sequential progressions in a theory-of-mind scale: Longitudinal perspectives. Child Development, 82, 780–792. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01583.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wellman, H. M. & Liu, D. (2004). Scaling of theory-of-mind tasks. Child Development, 75, 523–541. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00691.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Woolley, J. D. & Phelps, K. E. (2001). The development of children’s beliefs about prayer. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 1, 139–166. https://doi.org/10.1163/156853701316931380 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar