Skip to main content
Review Article

Improving Metacognition in the Classroom

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/2151-2604/a000440

Abstract. Students are often overconfident in educational settings and struggle to differentiate between well-learned and poorly-learned concepts. The present article reviews current research on strategies that help students assess their understanding, with a focus on research using authentic educational tasks and materials. We propose a framework for these strategies that we refer to as wait-generate-validate. The wait-generate-validate strategies can give students a more objective measure of their learning from lectures, understanding of course concepts, text comprehension, problem-solving ability, and test preparedness. These strategies have been shown to lead to more effective study decisions and greater learning. Lastly, we translate the reviewed research into practical tips for students and teachers and conclude with recommendations for future research regarding how students judge their learning in diverse educational contexts.

References

  • Ackerman, R., & Goldsmith, M. (2011). Metacognitive regulation of text learning: On screen versus on paper. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 17(1), 18–32. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022086 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ackerman, R., & Lauterman, T. (2012). Taking reading comprehension exams on screen or on paper? A metacognitive analysis of learning texts under time pressure. Computers in Human Behavior, 28(5), 1816–1828. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2012.04.023 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Aleven, V., Mclaren, B., Roll, I., & Koedinger, K. (2006). Toward meta-cognitive tutoring: A model of help seeking with a Cognitive Tutor. International Journal of Artificial Intelligence in Education, 16(2), 101–128. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Aleven, V., Roll, I., McLaren, B. M., & Koedinger, K. R. (2016). Help helps, but only so much: Research on help seeking with intelligent tutoring systems. International Journal of Artificial Intelligence in Education, 26(1), 205–223. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40593-015-0089-1 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Alter, A. L., Oppenheimer, D. M., & Zemla, J. C. (2010). Missing the trees for the forest: A construal level account of the illusion of explanatory depth. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99(3), 436–451. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020218 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Anderson, M. C., & Thiede, K. W. (2008). Why do delayed summaries improve metacomprehension accuracy? Acta Psychologica, 128(1), 110–118. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2007.10.006 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ariel, R., & Dunlosky, J. (2011). The sensitivity of judgment-of-learning resolution to past test performance, new learning, and forgetting. Memory & Cognition, 39(1), 171–184. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-010-0002-y First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Azevedo, R. (2005a). Using hypermedia as a metacognitive tool for enhancing student learning? The role of self-regulated learning. Educational Psychologist, 40(4), 199–209. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326985ep4004_2 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Azevedo, R. (2005b). Computer environments as metacognitive tools for enhancing learning. Educational Psychologist, 40(4), 193–197. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326985ep4004_1 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Azevedo, R., & Aleven, V. (2013). International handbook of metacognition and learning technologies (Vol. 26). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4419-5546-3 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Azevedo, R., & Cromley, J. G. (2004). Does training on self-regulated learning facilitate students’ learning with hypermedia? Journal of Educational Psychology, 96(3), 523–535. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.96.3.523 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Azevedo, R., & Hadwin, A. F. (2005). Scaffolding self-regulated learning and metacognition – Implications for the design of computer-based scaffolds. Instructional Science, 33(5/6), 367–379. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11251-005-1272-9 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Baars, M., Van Gog, T., de Bruin, A., & Paas, F. (2014). Effects of problem solving after worked example study on primary school children’s monitoring accuracy. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 28(3), 382–391. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.3008 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Baars, M., Van Gog, T., de Bruin, A., & Paas, F. (2017). Effects of problem solving after worked example study on secondary school children’s monitoring accuracy. Educational Psychology, 37(7), 810–834. https://doi.org/10.1080/01443410.2016.1150419 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Baars, M., Vink, S., Van Gog, T., De Bruin, A., & Paas, F. (2014). Effects of training self-assessment and using assessment standards on retrospective and prospective monitoring of problem solving. Learning and Instruction, 33, 92–107. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2014.04.004 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Baars, M., Visser, S., Van Gog, T., De Bruin, A., & Paas, F. (2013). Completion of partially worked-out examples as a generation strategy for improving monitoring accuracy. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 38(4), 395–406. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2013.09.001 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bahrick, H. P. (1979). Maintenance of knowledge: Questions about memory we forgot to ask. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 108(3), 296–308. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.108.3.296 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Baker, J. M., Dunlosky, J., & Hertzog, C. (2010). How accurately can older adults evaluate the quality of their text recall? The effect of providing standards on judgment accuracy. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 24(1), 134–147. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1553 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Benjamin, A. S., & Diaz, M. (2008). Measurement of relative metamnemonic accuracy. In J. DunloskyR. A. BjorkEds., Handbook of memory and metamemory (pp. 73–94). Psychology Press. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203805503 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Benjamin, A. S., Bjork, R. A., & Schwartz, B. L. (1998). The mismeasure of memory: When retrieval fluency is misleading as a metamnemonic index. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 127(1), 55–68. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.127.1.55 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bjork, R. A., Dunlosky, J., & Kornell, N. (2013). Self-regulated learning: Beliefs, techniques, and illusions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 417–444. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143823 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bol, L., & Hacker, D. J. (2001). A comparison of the effects of practice tests and traditional review on performance and calibration. The Journal of Experimental Education, 69(2), 133–151. https://doi.org/10.1080/00220970109600653 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bol, L., Hacker, D. J., O’Shea, P., & Allen, D. (2005). The influence of overt practice, achievement level, and explanatory style on calibration accuracy and performance. The Journal of Experimental Education, 73(4), 269–290. https://doi.org/10.3200/JEXE.73.4.269-290 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Brown, A., & Green, T. (2016). Virtual reality: Low-cost tools and resources for the classroom. TechTrends, 60(5), 517–519. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11528-016-0102-z First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Carpenter, S. K., Mickes, L., Rahman, S., & Fernandez, C. (2016). The effect of instructor fluency on students’ perceptions of instructors, confidence in learning, and actual learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 22(2), 161–172. https://doi.org/10.1037/xap0000077 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Carpenter, S. K., Wilford, M. M., Kornell, N., & Mullaney, K. M. (2013). Appearances can be deceiving: Instructor fluency increases perceptions of learning without increasing actual learning. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20(6), 1350–1356. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-013-0442-z First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cepeda, N. J., Pashler, H., Vul, E., Wixted, J. T., & Rohrer, D. (2006). Distributed practice in verbal recall tasks: A review and quantitative synthesis. Psychological Bulletin, 132(3), 354–380. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.132.3.354 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cuevas, H. M., Fiore, S. M., & Oser, R. L. (2002). Scaffolding cognitive and metacognitive processes in low verbal ability learners: Use of diagrams in computer-based training environments. Instructional Science, 30(6), 433–464. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1020516301541 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • de Bruin, A. B., Kok, E. M., Lobbestael, J., & de Grip, A. (2017). The impact of an online tool for monitoring and regulating learning at university: Overconfidence, learning strategy, and personality. Metacognition and Learning, 12(1), 21–43. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11409-016-9159-5 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • de Bruin, A. B., Thiede, K. W., Camp, G., & Redford, J. (2011). Generating keywords improves metacomprehension and self-regulation in elementary and middle school children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 109(3), 294–310. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2011.02.005 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dunlosky, J., & Lipko, A. R. (2007). Metacomprehension: A brief history and how to improve its accuracy. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(4), 228–232. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00509.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dunlosky, J., Mueller, M. L., & Thiede, K. W. (2016). Methodology for investigating human metamemory: Problems and pitfalls. In J. DunloskyS. K. TauberEds., The Oxford handbook of metamemory (pp. 23–37). Oxford University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Dunlosky, J., & Nelson, T. O. (1992). Importance of the kind of cue for judgments of learning (JOL) and the delayed-JOL effect. Memory & Cognition, 20(4), 374–380. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03210921 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dunlosky, J., & Nelson, T. O. (1997). Similarity between the cue for judgments of learning (JOL) and the cue for test is not the primary determinant of JOL accuracy. Journal of Memory and Language, 36(1), 34–49. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1996.2476 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dunlosky, J., & Rawson, K. A. (2012). Overconfidence produces underachievement: Inaccurate self evaluations undermine students’ learning and retention. Learning and Instruction, 22(4), 271–280. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2011.08.003 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dunlosky, J., Rawson, K. A., & McDonald, S. L. (2002). Influence of practice tests on the accuracy of predicting memory performance for paired associates, sentences, and text material. In T. J. PerfectB. L. SchwartzEds., Applied metacognition (pp. 68–92). Cambridge University Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dunlosky, J., Rawson, K. A., & Middleton, E. L. (2005). What constrains the accuracy of metacomprehension judgments? Testing the transfer-appropriate-monitoring and accessibility hypotheses. Journal of Memory and Language, 52(4), 551–565. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2005.01.011 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dunning, D., Heath, C., & Suls, J. M. (2004). Flawed self-assessment: Implications for health, education, and the workplace. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 5(3), 69–106. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1529-1006.2004.00018.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dutke, S., Barenberg, J., & Leopold, C. (2010). Learning from text: Knowing the test format enhanced metacognitive monitoring. Metacognition and Learning, 5(2), 195–206. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11409-010-9057-1 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Falchikov, N., & Boud, D. (1989). Student self-assessment in higher education: A meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research, 59(4), 395–430. https://doi.org/10.3102/00346543059004395 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Feyzi-Behnagh, R., & Azevedo, R. (2012). The effectiveness of a pedagogical agent’s immediate feedback on learners’ metacognitive judgments during learning with MetaTutor. In S. A. CerriW. J. ClanceyG. PapadourakisK. PanourgiaEds., International Conference on Intelligent Tutoring Systems (pp. 651–652). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-30950-2_105 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Feyzi-Behnagh, R., Azevedo, R., Legowski, E., Reitmeyer, K., Tseytlin, E., & Crowley, R. S. (2014). Metacognitive scaffolds improve self-judgments of accuracy in a medical intelligent tutoring system. Instructional Science, 42(2), 159–181. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11251-013-9275-4 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Finn, B., & Metcalfe, J. (2007). The role of memory for past test in the underconfidence with practice effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33(1), 238–244. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.33.1.238 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Finn, B., & Metcalfe, J. (2008). Judgments of learning are influenced by memory for past test. Journal of Memory and Language, 58(1), 19–34. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2007.03.006 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Finn, B., & Tauber, S. K. (2015). When confidence is not a signal of knowing: How students’ experiences and beliefs about processing fluency can lead to miscalibrated confidence. Educational Psychology Review, 27(4), 567–586. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-015-9313-7 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Flavell, J. H. (1979). Metacognition and cognitive monitoring: A new area of cognitive–developmental inquiry. American Psychologist, 34(10), 906–911. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.34.10.906 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Foster, N. L., Was, C. A., Dunlosky, J., & Isaacson, R. M. (2017). Even after thirteen class exams, students are still overconfident: The role of memory for past exam performance in student predictions. Metacognition and Learning, 12(1), 1–19. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11409-016-9158-6 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fukaya, T. (2013). Explanation generation, not explanation expectancy, improves metacomprehension accuracy. Metacognition and Learning, 8(1), 1–18. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11409-012-9093-0 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Glenberg, A. M., & Epstein, W. (1985). Calibration of comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 11(4), 702–718. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.11.1-4.702 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Glenberg, A. M., & Epstein, W. (1987). Inexpert calibration of comprehension. Memory & Cognition, 15, 84–93. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03197714 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Graesser, A. C., Conley, M. W., & Olney, A. (2012). Intelligent tutoring systems. In K. R. HarrisS. GrahamT. UrdanA. G. BusS. MajorH. L. SwansonEds., APA handbooks in psychology®. APA educational psychology handbook, Vol. 3. Application to learning and teaching (pp. 451–473). American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/13275-018 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Griffin, T., Mielicki, M., & Wiley, J. (2019). Improving students’ metacomprehension accuracy. In J. DunloskyK. RawsonEds., The Cambridge handbook of cognition and education (Cambridge Handbooks in Psychology, pp. 619–646). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108235631.025 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Griffin, T. D., Wiley, J., & Salas, C. R. (2013). Supporting effective self-regulated learning: The critical role of monitoring. In R. AzevedoV. AlevenEds., International handbook of metacognition and learning technologies (pp. 19–34). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4419-5546-3_2 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Griffin, T. D., Wiley, J., & Thiede, K. W. (2008). Individual differences, rereading, and self-explanation: Concurrent processing and cue validity as constraints on metacomprehension accuracy. Memory & Cognition, 36(1), 93–103. https://doi.org/10.3758/MC.36.1.93 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Grimes, P. W. (2002). The overconfident principles of economics student: An examination of a metacognitive skill. The Journal of Economic Education, 33(1), 15–30. https://doi.org/10.1080/00220480209596121 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hacker, D., & Bol, L. (2019). Calibration and self-regulated learning: Making the connections. In J. DunloskyK. RawsonEds., The Cambridge handbook of cognition and education (Cambridge Handbooks in Psychology, pp. 647–677). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108235631.026 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hacker, D. J., Bol, L., & Bahbahani, K. (2008). Explaining calibration accuracy in classroom contexts: The effects of incentives, reflection, and explanatory style. Metacognition and Learning, 3(2), 101–121. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11409-008-9021-5 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hacker, D. J., Bol, L., & Keener, M. C. (2008). Metacognition in education: A focus on calibration. In J. DunloskyR. A. BjorkEds., Handbook of metamemory and memory (pp. 429–455). Psychological Press. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203805503 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hacker, D. J., Bol, L., Horgan, D. D., & Rakow, E. A. (2000). Test prediction and performance in a classroom context. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92(1), 160–170. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.92.1.160 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Halamish, V., & Elbaz, E. (2020). Children’s reading comprehension and metacomprehension on screen versus on paper. Computers & Education, 145, Article 103737. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2019.103737 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Händel, M., Harder, B., & Dresel, M. (2020). Enhanced monitoring accuracy and test performance: Incremental effects of judgment training over and above repeated testing. Learning and Instruction, 65, Article 101245. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2019.101245 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hartwig, M. K., & Dunlosky, J. (2017). Category learning judgments in the classroom: Can students judge how well they know course topics? Contemporary Educational Psychology, 49, 80–90. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2016.12.002 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hattie, J., & Timperley, H. (2007). The power of feedback. Review of Educational Research, 77(1), 81–112. https://doi.org/10.3102/003465430298487 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jacoby, L. L., Wahlheim, C. N., & Coane, J. H. (2010). Test-enhanced learning of natural concepts: Effects on recognition memory, classification, and metacognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36(6), 1441–1451. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020636 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jaeger, A. J., & Wiley, J. (2014). Do illustrations help or harm metacomprehension accuracy? Learning and Instruction, 34, 58–73. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2014.08.002 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Karjanto, N., & Yong, S. T. (2013). Test anxiety in mathematics among early undergraduate students in a British university in Malaysia. European Journal of Engineering Education, 38(1), 11–37. https://doi.org/10.1080/03043797.2012.742867 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Karpicke, J. D. (2009). Metacognitive control and strategy selection: Deciding to practice retrieval during learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 138(4), 469–486. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017341 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kelemen, W. L., & Weaver, C. A. III (1997). Enhanced memory at delays: Why do judgments of learning improve over time? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23(6), 1394–1409. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.23.6.1394 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kimball, D. R., Smith, T. A., & Muntean, W. J. (2012). Does delaying judgments of learning really improve the efficacy of study decisions? Not so much. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38(4), 923–954. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Koriat, A. (1997). Monitoring one’s own knowledge during study: A cue-utilization approach to judgments of learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 126(4), 349–370. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.126.4.349 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Koriat, A. (2012). The relationships between monitoring, regulation and performance. Learning and Instruction, 22(4), 296–298. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2012.01.002 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kornell, N., & Bjork, R. A. (2008). Learning concepts and categories: Is spacing the “enemy of induction”? Psychological Science, 19(6), 585–592. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02127.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kornell, N., & Finn, B. (2016). Self-regulated learning: An overview of theory and data. In J. DunloskyS. K. TauberEds., The Oxford handbook of metamemory (pp. 325–340). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199336746.013.23 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kornell, N., & Rhodes, M. G. (2013). Feedback reduces the metacognitive benefit of tests. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 19(1), 1–13. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032147 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lipko, A. R., Dunlosky, J., Hartwig, M. K., Rawson, K. A., Swan, K., & Cook, D. (2009). Using standards to improve middle school students’ accuracy at evaluating the quality of their recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 15(4), 307–318. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017599 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lipko, A. R., Dunlosky, J., & Merriman, W. E. (2009). Persistent overconfidence despite practice: The role of task experience in preschoolers’ recall predictions. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 103(2), 152–166. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2008.10.002 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Little, J. L., & McDaniel, M. A. (2015). Individual differences in category learning: Memorization versus rule abstraction. Memory & Cognition, 43(2), 283–297. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-014-0475-1 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Maki, R. H. (1998). Predicting performance on text: Delayed versus immediate predictions and tests. Memory & Cognition, 26(5), 959–964. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03201176 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Masson, M. E., & Rotello, C. M. (2009). Sources of bias in the Goodman–Kruskal gamma coefficient measure of association: Implications for studies of metacognitive processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35(2), 509–527. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014876 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McDaniel, M. A., Howard, D. C., & Einstein, G. O. (2009). The read-recite-review study strategy: Effective and portable. Psychological Science, 20(4), 516–522. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02325.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Metcalfe, J. (2009). Metacognitive judgments and control of study. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18(3), 159–163. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01628.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Miller, T. M., & Geraci, L. (2014). Improving metacognitive accuracy: How failing to retrieve practice items reduces overconfidence. Consciousness and Cognition, 29, 131–140. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2014.08.008 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Miller, T. M., & Geraci, L. (2016). The influence of retrieval practice on metacognition: The contribution of analytic and non-analytic processes. Consciousness and Cognition, 42, 41–50. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2016.03.010 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mitsuda, M. (1988). Effects of processing variables on comprehension monitoring activities. Psychologia: An International Journal of Psychology in the Orient, 31(2), 98–105. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Mudrick, N. V., Sawyer, R., Price, M. J., Lester, J., Roberts, C., & Azevedo, R. (2018). Identifying how metacognitive judgments influence student performance during learning with MetaTutorIVH. In R. NkambouR. AzevedoJ. VassilevaEds., International Conference on Intelligent Tutoring Systems (pp. 140–149). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-91464-0_14 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Nelson, T. O. (1984). A comparison of current measures of the accuracy of feeling-of-knowing predictions. Psychology Bulletin, 95(1), 109–133. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.95.1.109 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Nelson, T. O. (1996). Consciousness and metacognition. American Psychologist, 51(2), 102–116. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.51.2.102 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Nelson, T. O., & Narens, L. (1990). Metamemory: A theoretical framework and new findings. The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 26, 125–141. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-7421(08)60053-5 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Nietfeld, J. L., Cao, L., & Osborne, J. W. (2005). Metacognitive monitoring accuracy and student performance in the postsecondary classroom. The Journal of Experimental Educational, 74(1), 7–28. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Nietfeld, J. L., Cao, L., & Osborne, J. W. (2006). The effect of distributed monitoring exercises and feedback on performance, monitoring accuracy, and self-efficacy. Metacognition and Learning, 1(2), 159–179. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10409-006-9595-6 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Prinz, A., Golke, S., & Wittwer, J. (2018). The double curse of misconceptions: Misconceptions impair not only text comprehension but also metacomprehension in the domain of statistics. Instructional Science, 46(5), 723–765. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11251-018-9452-6 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rawson, K. A., & Dunlosky, J. (2002). Are performance predictions for text based on ease of processing? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28(1), 69–80. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.28.1.69 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rawson, K. A., & Dunlosky, J. (2007). Improving students’ self-evaluation of learning for key concepts in textbook materials. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 19(4–5), 559–579. https://doi.org/10.1080/09541440701326022 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rawson, K. A., & Dunlosky, J. (2013). Retrieval-monitoring-feedback (RMF) technique for producing efficient and durable student learning. In R. AzevedoV. AlevenEds., International handbook of metacognition and learning technologies (pp. 67–78). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4419-5546-3_5 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rawson, K. A., Dunlosky, J., & Sciartelli, S. M. (2013). The power of successive relearning: Improving performance on course exams and long-term retention. Educational Psychology Review, 25(4), 523–548. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-013-9240-4 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rawson, K. A., O’Neil, R., & Dunlosky, J. (2011). Accurate monitoring leads to effective control and greater learning of patient education materials. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 17(3), 288–302. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024749 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Redford, J. S., Thiede, K. W., Wiley, J., & Griffin, T. D. (2012). Concept mapping improves metacomprehension accuracy among 7th graders. Learning and Instruction, 22(4), 262–270. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2011.10.007 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rhodes, M. G. (2016). Judgments of learning: Methods, data, and theory. In J. DunloskyS. K. TauberEds., The Oxford handbook of metamemory (pp. 65–80). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199336746.013.4 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rhodes, M. G. (2019). Metacognition. Teaching of Psychology, 46(2), 168–175. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199336746.013.4 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rhodes, M. G., Cleary, A. M., & DeLosh, E. L. (2020). A guide to effective studying and learning: Practical strategies from the science of learning, Oxford University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Rhodes, M. G., & Tauber, S. K. (2011). The influence of delaying judgments of learning on metacognitive accuracy: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 137(1), 131–148. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021705 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Roll, I., Aleven, V., McLaren, B. M., Ryu, E., Baker, R. S., & Koedinger, K. R. (2006). The help tutor: Does metacognitive feedback improve students’ help-seeking actions, skills and learning?. In M. IkedaK. D. AshleyT. W. ChanEds., International Conference on Intelligent Tutoring Systems (pp. 360–369). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/11774303_36 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rowland, C. A. (2014). The effect of testing versus restudy on retention: A meta-analytic review of the testing effect. Psychological Bulletin, 140(6), 1432–1463. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037559 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rozenblit, L., & Keil, F. (2002). The misunderstood limits of folk science: An illusion of explanatory depth. Cognitive Science, 26(5), 521–562. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog2605_1 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rutten, N., Van Joolingen, W. R., & Van Der Veen, J. T. (2012). The learning effects of computer simulations in science education. Computers & Education, 58(1), 136–153. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2011.07.017 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Saenz, G. D., Geraci, L., & Tirso, R. (2019). Improving metacognition: A comparison of interventions. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 33(5), 918–929. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.3556 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Scheck, P., Meeter, M., & Nelson, T. O. (2004). Anchoring effects in the absolute accuracy of immediate versus delayed judgments of learning. Journal of Memory and Language, 51(1), 71–79. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2004.03.004 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schraw, G. (2009). A conceptual analysis of five measures of metacognitive monitoring. Metacognition and Learning, 4(1), 33–45. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11409-008-9031-3 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Serra, M. J., & Dunlosky, J. (2010). Metacomprehension judgements reflect the belief that diagrams improve learning from text. Memory, 18(7), 698–711. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2010.506441 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sidi, Y., Shpigelman, M., Zalmanov, H., & Ackerman, R. (2017). Understanding metacognitive inferiority on screen by exposing cues for depth of processing. Learning and Instruction, 51, 61–73. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2017.01.002 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Szpunar, K. K., Jing, H. G., & Schacter, D. L. (2014). Overcoming overconfidence in learning from video-recorded lectures: Implications of interpolated testing for online education. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 3(3), 161–164. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2014.02.001 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Thiede, K. W., & Anderson, M. C. (2003). Summarizing can improve metacomprehension accuracy. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 28(2), 129–160. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0361-476X(02)00011-5 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Thiede, K. W., Anderson, M., & Therriault, D. (2003). Accuracy of metacognitive monitoring affects learning of texts. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95(1), 66–73. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.95.1.66 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Thiede, K. W., & Dunlosky, J. (1994). Delaying students’ metacognitive monitoring improves their accuracy in predicting their recognition performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86(2), 290–302. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.86.2.290 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Thiede, K. W., Dunlosky, J., Griffin, T. D., & Wiley, J. (2005). Understanding the delayed-keyword effect on metacomprehension accuracy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31(6), 1267–1280. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.31.6.1267 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Thiede, K. W., Griffin, T. D., Wiley, J., & Anderson, M. C. (2010). Poor metacomprehension accuracy as a result of inappropriate cue use. Discourse Processes, 47(4), 331–362. https://doi.org/10.1080/01638530902959927 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Thiede, K. W., Griffin, T. D., Wiley, J., & Redford, J. S. (2009). Metacognitive monitoring during and after reading. In Handbook of metacognition in education (pp. 97–118). Routledge. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Thiede, K. W., Redford, J. S., Wiley, J., & Griffin, T. D. (2012). Elementary school experience with comprehension testing may influence metacomprehension accuracy among seventh and eighth graders. Journal of Educational Psychology, 104(3), 554–564. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028660 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Thiede, K. W., Wiley, J., & Griffin, T. D. (2011). Test expectancy affects metacomprehension accuracy. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 81(2), 264–273. https://doi.org/10.1348/135910710X510494 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Thomas, R. C., Finn, B., & Jacoby, L. L. (2016). Prior experience shapes metacognitive judgments at the category level: The role of testing and category difficulty. Metacognition and Learning, 11(3), 257–274. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11409-015-9144-4 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Thomas, A. K., & McDaniel, M. A. (2007a). The negative cascade of incongruent generative study-test processing in memory and metacomprehension. Memory & Cognition, 35(4), 668–678. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Thomas, A. K., & McDaniel, M. A. (2007b). Metacomprehension for educationally relevant materials: Dramatic effects of encoding-retrieval interactions. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14(2), 212–218. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193305 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Van Loon, M. H., de Bruin, A. B., Van Gog, T., & Van Merriënboer, J. J. (2013a). Activation of inaccurate prior knowledge affects primary-school students’ metacognitive judgments and calibration. Learning and Instruction, 24, 15–25. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2012.08.005 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Van Loon, M. H., De Bruin, A. B., Van Gog, T., & Van Merriënboer, J. J. (2013b). The effect of delayed-JOLs and sentence generation on children’s monitoring accuracy and regulation of idiom study. Metacognition and Learning, 8(2), 173–191. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11409-013-9100-0 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Van Loon, M. H., De Bruin, A. B., Van Gog, T., Van Merriënboer, J. J., & Dunlosky, J. (2014). Can students evaluate their understanding of cause-and-effect relations? The effects of diagram completion on monitoring accuracy. Acta Psychologica, 151, 143–154. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.06.007 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Von der Linden, N., Löffler, E., & Schneider, W. (2017). Relation of metacognitive monitoring and control processes across the life-span. Journal of Educational and Developmental Psychology, 7(1), 86–101. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Walczyk, J. J., & Hall, V. C. (1989). Effects of examples and embedded questions on the accuracy of comprehension self-assessments. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81(3), 435–437. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.81.3.435 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wiley, J., Griffin, T. D., Jaeger, A. J., Jarosz, A. F., Cushen, P. J., & Thiede, K. W. (2016). Improving metacomprehension accuracy in an undergraduate course context. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 22(4), 393–405. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wiley, J., Jaeger, A. J., Taylor, A. R., & Griffin, T. D. (2018). When analogies harm: The effects of analogies on metacomprehension. Learning and Instruction, 55, 113–123. https://doi.org/10.1037/xap0000096 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Winne, P. H., & Jamieson-Noel, D. (2002). Exploring students’ calibration of self reports about study tactics and achievement. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 27(4), 551–572. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0361-476X(02)00006-1 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wissman, K. T., Rawson, K. A., & Pyc, M. A. (2012). How and when do students use flashcards? Memory, 20(6), 568–579. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2012.687052 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Zamary, A., Rawson, K. A., & Dunlosky, J. (2016). How accurately can students evaluate the quality of self-generated examples of declarative concepts? Not well, and feedback does not help. Learning and Instruction, 46, 12–20. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2016.08.002 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar