Skip to main content
Editorial

Schulische Vorläufer: Bildungsgerechtigkeit beginnt vor der Einschulung!

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1024/2235-0977/a000378
Free first page

Literatur

  • Burr, D. C. , Anobile, G. , & Arrighi, R. (2017). Psychophysical evidence for the number sense. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society. Series B, Biological sciences , 373 (1740), 20170045. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Clausen-Suhr, K. & Walter, J. (2022). Screening-Verfahren zur Prognose von Rechenschwierigkeiten in der Grundschule. Lernen und Lernstörungen , 11 , 125–138. First citation in articleAbstractGoogle Scholar

  • Hannagan, T. , Nieder, A. , Viswanathan, P. & Dehaene, S. (2017). A random-matrix theory f the number sense. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B , 373 : 20170253. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jordan, N. C. , Kaplan, D. , Ramineni, C. & Locuniak, M. N. (2009). Early math matters: Kindergarten number competence and later mathematics outcomes. Developmental Psychology , 45 , 850–867. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Nguyen, T. , Watts, T. W. , Duncan, G .J. , Clements, D. H. , Sarama, J. S. , Wolfe, C. & Spitler, M. E. (2016). Which preschool mathematics competencies are most predictive of fifth grade achievement? Early Child Research Quarterly , 36 , 550–560. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Nieder, A. (2016). The neural code for number. Nature Reviews Neuroscience , 17 , 366–382. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • OECD (2011) Inclusion of Students with Disabilities in Tertiary Education and Employment, Education and Training Policy, OECD Publishing, Paris , https://doi.org/10.1787/9789264097650-en. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Parsons, S. & Bynner, B. (2005). Does Numeracy Matter More? National Research and Development Centre for Adult Literacy and Numeracy : London, UK. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Roesch, S. , Bahnmueller, J. , Barrocas, R. & Moeller, K. (2022). Feinmotorik, Fingergnosie und frühe mathematische Fähigkeiten: Ein Überblick. Lernen und Lernstörungen , 11 , 139–157. First citation in articleAbstractGoogle Scholar

  • Siegler, R. S. (2016). Magnitude knowledge: The common core of numerical development. Developmental Science , 19 , 341 361. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Spilles, M. (2022). Cliffhanger als gewinnbringendes Stilmittel bei der Leseförderung? Lernen und Lernstörungen , 11 , 158–166. First citation in articleAbstractGoogle Scholar

  • Träff, U. (2013). The contribution of general cognitive abilities and number abilities to different aspects of mathematics in children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology , 116 , 139–156. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Van Bueren, N. E. R. , van der Ven, S. H. G. , Roelofs, K. , Cohen Kadosh, R. & Kroesbergen, E. H. (2022). Predicting math ability using working memory, and neurophysiology in children and adults. Brain Sciences , 12 : 550. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • von Aster, M. , Kaufmann, L. , McCaskey, U. & Kucian, K. (2021) Rechenstörungen im Kindes- und Jugendalter. In Fegert J. et al. (Hrsg.) Psychiatrie und Psychotherapie des Kindes- und Jugendalters . Springer Reference Medizin. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Weiss, E. , Kaufmann, L. , Ninaus, M. & Canazei, M. (2022). Belastungen durch Fernlehre und psychische Gesundheit von Studierenden während der COVID-19-Pandemie. Lernen und Lernstörungen , 11 , 167–179. First citation in articleAbstractGoogle Scholar