Skip to main content
Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1026/0012-1924/a000244

Zusammenfassung. Das Emotionswissen von Kindern umfasst neben dem Ablesen von Emotionen aus Gesichtern anderer Menschen auch das Erkennen von externalen und internalen Ursachen von Emotionen sowie das Wissen über ihre Konsequenzen und Regulationsmöglichkeiten. Im deutschsprachigen Raum gibt es bisher kein Testverfahren, welches das Emotionswissen auf diese Weise differenziert und gleichzeitig reliabel und valide erfasst. Mit dem Adaptiven Test des Emotionswissens (ATEM) wird diese Lücke geschlossen. Der ATEM fragt in einem adaptiven Design 6 Teilaspekte des Emotionswissens ab, wobei die 32 Items in eine kinderfreundliche Geschichte eingebettet sind. Der nach der Item-Response-Theorie aufgebaute Test zeigt mit einer Gesamtstichprobe von N = 581 Kindern im Alter zwischen 3 und 9 Jahren gute psychometrische Eigenschaften. In einer Substichprobe von n = 254 Kindern im Alter zwischen 3 und 5 Jahren wurde der ATEM anhand von Korrelationen mit verschiedenen Variablen validiert. Insgesamt eignet sich der ATEM gut zur differenzierten Messung des Emotionswissens bei Kindern.


Validation of the Adaptive Test of Emotion Knowledge (ATEM) for 3 – 9-Year-Olds

Abstract. Emotion knowledge in children includes emotion recognition in other peoples’ faces, as well as understanding the internal and external causes and consequences of emotions and strategies for regulating them. To date, there is no measure in the German-speaking countries that captures emotion knowledge in a differentiated but also reliable and valid way. The Adaptive Test of Emotion Knowledge (ATEM) aims to close this gap. The ATEM follows an adaptive design and six components of emotion knowledge. The 32 Rasch-scaled items are embedded in an attractive story and vary in their difficulty within and between the components. In a sample of 581 3 – 9-year-olds, the test showed a good fit to a six-dimensional model and very good psychometric properties. In a subsample of 254 3 – 5-year-olds, correlations between the ATEM and a measure of emotion knowledge and measures of expressive and receptive language abilities demonstrated good validity. The ATEM proved to be a good measure of emotion knowledge in children.

Literatur

  • Adams, R. J. & Wu, M. L. (2003). PISA Programme for International Student Assessment (PISA). PISA 2000 Technical Report: OECD Publishing. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Agnoli, S., Mancini, G., Pozzoli, T., Baldaro, B., Russo, P. M. & Surcinelli, P. (2012). The interaction between emotional intelligence and cognitive ability in predicting scholastic performance in school-aged children. Personality and Individual Differences, 53, 660 – 665. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2012.05.020 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Best, J. R., Miller, P. H. & Naglieri, J. A. (2011). Relations between executive function and academic achievement from ages 5 to 17 in a large, representative national sample. Learning and Individual Differences, 21, 327 – 336. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2011.01.007 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bulheller, S. & Häcker, H. (2006). Raven’s Coloured Progressive Matrices. Frankfurt am Main: Pearson. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Camras, L. A. & Allison, K. (1985). Children’s understanding of emotional facial expressions and verbal labels. Journal of nonverbal Behavior, 9, 84 – 94. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00987140 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Castro, V. L., Halberstadt, A. G. & Garrett-Peters, P. (2016). A three-factor structure of emotion understanding in third-grade children. Social Development, 25, 602 – 622. https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12162 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale: L. Erlbaum Associates. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Cole, P. M. (1986). Children’s spontaneous control of facial expression. Child Development, 57, 1309 – 1321. https://doi.org/10.2307/1130411 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cutting, A. L. & Dunn, J. (1999). Theory of mind, emotion understanding, language, and family background. Individual differences and interrelations. Child Development, 70, 853 – 865. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00061 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Denham, S. A. (1998). Emotional development in young children (The Guilford series on social and emotional development). New York: Guilford Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Ekman, P. & Friesen, W. V. (1978). Facial Action Coding System. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologist Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Fisseni, H. J. (2004). Lehrbuch der psychologischen Diagnostik. Mit Hinweisen zur Intervention (3., überarb. und erw. Aufl.). Göttingen: Hogrefe. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Garner, P. W. & Waajid, B. (2012). Emotion knowledge and self-regulation as predictors of preschoolers’ cognitive ability, classroom behavior, and social competence. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 30, 330 – 343. https://doi.org/10.1177/0734282912449441 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gestsdottir, S., Suchodoletz, A., Wanless, S. B., Hubert, B., Guimard, P., Birgisdottir, F. et al. (2014). Early behavioral self-regulation, academic achievement, and gender. Applied Developmental Science, 18, 90 – 109. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888691.2014.894870 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Grimm, H., Aktaş, M. & Frevert, S. (2015). SETK 3 – 5: Sprachentwicklungstest für drei- bis fünfjährige Kinder (3;0 – 5;11 Jahre). Diagnose von Sprachverarbeitungsfähigkeiten und auditiven Gedächtnisleistungen (3., überarbeitete und neu normierte Aufl.). Göttingen: Hogrefe. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Grob, A., Meyer, C. S. & Hagmann-von Arx, P. (2009). Intelligenz- und Entwicklungsskalen für Kinder von 5 – 10 Jahren (2. Aufl.). Bern: Huber. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Harris, P. L., Johnson, C. N., Hutton, D., Andrews, G. & Cooke, T. (1989). Young children’s theory of mind and emotion. Cognition & Emotion, 3, 379 – 400. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699938908412713 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Holodynski, M. (2004). The miniaturization of expression in the development of emotional self-regulation. Developmental Psychology, 40, 16 – 28. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.40.1.16 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • In-Albon, T. & Pfeiffer, S. (2015). Messverfahren für emotionale Kompetenz bei Kindern im Vor- und Grundschulalter. Handbuch Installation und Anwendung. Koblenz-Landau: Campus Landau. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Janke, B. (2006). Emotion knowledge scale for 3- to 10- year-old children. Skala zur Erfassung des Emotionswissens für 3- bis 10-jährige Kinder. Heidelberg: Pädagogische Hochschule. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Kårstad, S. B., Kvello, Ø., Wichstrøm, L. & Berg-Nielsen, T. S. (2014). What do parents know about their children’s comprehension of emotions? Accuracy of parental estimates in a community sample of pre-schoolers. Child: care, health and development, 40, 346 – 353. https://doi.org/10.1111/cch.12071 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Keller, M., Lourenço, O., Malti, T. & Saalbach, H. (2003). The multifaceted phenomenon of ‘happy victimizers’: A cross‐cultural comparison of moral emotions. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 21, 1 – 18. https://doi.org/10.1348/026151003321164582 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kiese-Himmel, C. (2005). AWST-R. Aktiver Wortschatztest für 3- bis 5-jährige Kinder – Revision. Göttingen: Beltz Test. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Kromm, H., Färber, M. & Holodynski, M. (2015). Felt or false smiles? Volitional regulation of emotional expression in 4-, 6-, and 8-year-old children. Child Development, 86, 579 – 597. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12315 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lagattuta, K. H. & Wellman, H. M. (2002). Differences in early parent-child conversations about negative versus positive emotions: Implications for the development of psychological understanding. Developmental Psychology, 38, 564 – 580. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.38.4.564 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lane, J. D., Wellman, H. M., Olson, S. L., LaBounty, J. & Kerr, D. C. R. (2010). Theory of mind and emotion understanding predict moral development in early childhood. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 28, 871 – 889. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lenhard, A., Lenhard, W., Segerer, R. & Suggate, S. P. (2015). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (4. Aufl.). Frankfurt am Main: Pearson. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Morra, S., Parrella, I. & Camba, R. (2011). The role of working memory in the development of emotion comprehension. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 29, 744 – 764. https://doi.org/10.1348/2044-835X.002006 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Petermann, F. & Gust, N. (2016). EMK 3 – 6. Inventar zur Erfassung emotionaler Kompetenzen bei Drei- bis Sechsjährigen. Göttingen: Hogrefe. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Petermann, F. & Wiedebusch, S. (2016). Emotionale Kompetenz bei Kindern. Göttingen: Hogrefe. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pons, F. & Harris, P. L. (2000). Test of Emotion Comprehension. Oxford: Oxford University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Pons, F., Harris, P. L. & Doudin, P.-A. (2002). Teaching emotion understanding. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 17, 293 – 304. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03173538 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pons, F., Harris, P. L. & Rosnay, M. de. (2004). Emotion comprehension between 3 and 11 years: Developmental periods and hierarchical organization. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 1, 127 – 152. https://doi.org/10.1080/17405620344000022 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • R Core Team. (2013). R: A language and environment for statistical computing (Version 13.1) [Computer software]. Vienna, Austria. Verfügbar unter http://www.R-project.org/. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Rost, J. (2004). Lehrbuch Testtheorie – Testkonstruktion (Psychologie Lehrbuch, 2., vollst. überarb. und erw. Aufl.). Bern: Huber. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Salisch, M. von, Denham, S. A. & Koch, T. (2017). Emotion knowledge and attention problems in young children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 45, 45 – 56. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-016-0157-5 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Salisch, M. von, Hänel, M. & Denham, S. A. (2015). Emotionswissen, exekutive Funktionen und Veränderungen bei Aufmerksamkeitsproblemen von Vorschulkindern. Kindheit und Entwicklung, 24, 78 – 85. https://doi.org/10.1026/0942-5403/a000163 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Salisch, M. von, Hänel, M. & Freund, P. A. (2013). Emotion understanding and cognitive abilities in young children. Learning and Individual Differences, 26, 15 – 19. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2013.04.001 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tenenbaum, H. R., Alfieri, L., Brooks, P. J. & Dunne, G. (2008). The effects of explanatory conversations on children’s emotion understanding. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 26, 249 – 263. https://doi.org/10.1348/026151007X231057 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Trentacosta, C. J. & Fine, S. E. (2010). Emotion knowledge, social competence, and behavior problems in childhood and adolescence: A meta-analytic review. Social Development, 19, 1 – 29. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2009.00543.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Voltmer, K. & Salisch, M. von. (2017). Three meta-analyses of children’s emotion knowledge and their school success. Learning and Individual Differences, 59, 107 – 118. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2017.08.006 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Voltmer, K. & Salisch, M. von. (2019). Native-born German and immigrant children’s development of emotion knowledge: A latent growth curve analysis. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 37, 112 – 129. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjdp.12262 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wang, W. C., Chen, P. H. & Cheng, Y. Y. (2004). Improving measurement precision of test batteries using multidimensional item response models. Psychological Methods, 9, 116 – 136. https://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.9.1.116 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wellman, H. M. & Liu, D. (2004). Scaling of theory-of-mind tasks. Child Development, 75, 523 – 541. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00691.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Widen, S. C. & Russell, J. A. (2003). A closer look at preschoolers’ freely produced labels for facial expressions. Developmental Psychology, 39, 114 – 128. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.39.1.114 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Widen, S. C. & Russell, J. A. (2013). Children’s recognition of disgust in others. Psychological Bulletin, 139, 271 – 299. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031640 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wintre, M. G. & Vallance, D. D. (1994). A developmental sequence in the comprehension of emotions: Intensity, multiple emotions, and valence. Developmental Psychology, 30, 509 – 514. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.30.4.509 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wu, M. L., Adams, R. J., Wilson, M. R. & Haldane, S. A. (2007). ACER ConQuest version 2.0. Generalised item response modelling software. Camberwell, Vic.: ACER Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar