Skip to main content
Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1026/0033-3042/a000174

Der Beitrag betrachtet das Wechselspiel zwischen Emotion und Emotionsregulation aus einer entwicklungspsychologischen Perspektive und bettet es in ein System an psychischen Regulationsformen ein. Dabei wird in kritischer Diskussion bestehender Konzeptionen zur Emotionsregulation eine emotionale und eine volitionale Form der Regulation von Handlungen einer reflexiven Form der Emotionsregulation gegenübergestellt. Letztere beinhaltet eine volitionale Regulation eigener Emotionen und ist erstmals im Laufe des Vorschulalters zu beobachten. Der Beitrag erläutert die Entwicklung der erforderlichen Kompetenzen, mit denen Kinder und Jugendliche ihre Emotionen in sozialkoordinierter und sozial akzeptierter Art und Weise regulieren können. Diese umfassen die Aneignung von Regulationsstrategien, die Entwicklung der exekutiven Funktionen, die Aneignung von Sprache als Mittel der psychologischen Distanzierung sowie mentales Zeitreisen.


Developmental basis of emotion regulation

This article approaches the interrelation between emotions and emotion regulation from a developmental perspective, embedding it in a system of psychological forms of regulation. Existing conceptions of emotion regulation are critically reviewed by contrasting an emotional and a volitional form of action regulation with a reflective form of emotion regulation. Reflective emotion regulation requires a volitional regulation of one’s own emotions and can first be observed at preschool age. This article describes the state of the art concerning the ontogenetic development of competences that children acquire in order to regulate their emotions in a socially coordinated and accepted way. The required competences are the acquisition of regulation strategies, the development of executive functions, the acquisition of language as a means of gaining psychological distance, and being able to engage in mental time travel.

Literatur

  • Achtziger, A. & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2006). Motivation und Volition im Handlungsverlauf. In J. Heckhausen & H. Heckhausen (Hrsg.), Motivation und Handeln (S. 277 – 302). Berlin: Springer. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Atance, C. M. (2008). Future thinking in young children. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17, 295 – 298. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Baaken, U. (2005). Der Weg des Kindes zum Kommunikationskünstler: Die Entwicklung der Ausdruckskontrolle bei vier- bis achtjährigen Kindern. Göttingen: Cuvillier. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Barnow, S. (2012). Emotionsregulation und Psychopathologie: Ein Überblick. Psychologische Rundschau, 63, 111 – 124. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Barrett, K. C. (1998). A functionalist perspective to the development of emotions. In M. F. Mascolo & S. Griffin (Eds.), What develops in emotional development? (pp. 109 – 133). New York: Plenum Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Barrett, K. C. (2012). Adaptive and maladaptive regulation of and by emotion: Process, context, and relation to self-regulation. In K. C. Barrett, N. A. Fox , G. A. Morgan, D. Fidler & L. Daunhauer (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulatory processes in development: New directions and international perspectives (pp. 61 – 78). New York: Psychology Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Bischof-Köhler, D. (2000). Kinder auf Zeitreise. Theory of Mind, Zeitverständnis und Handlungsorganisation. Bern: Huber. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Blair, K. A. , Denham, S. A. , Kochanoff, A. & Whipple, B. (2004). Playing it cool: Temperament, emotion regulation, and social behavior in preschoolers. Journal of School Psychology, 42, 419 – 443. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bohart, A. C. & Stipek, D. J. (2001). Constructive & destructive behavior: Implications for family, school, & society. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Botvinick, M. M. , Braver, T. S. , Barch, D. M. , Carter, C. S. & Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review, 108, 624 – 652. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bridges, L. J. & Grolnick, W. S. (1995). The development of emotional self-regulation in infancy and early childhood. In N. Eisenberg (Ed.), Social development (pp. 185 – 211). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Broderick, N. Y. (2001). An investigation of the relationship between private speech and emotion regulation in preschool-age children. 61, ProQuest Information & Learning, US. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Brown, T. & Kozak, A. (1998). Emotion and the possibility of psychologists entering into heaven. In M. F. Mascolo & S. Griffin (Eds.), What develops in emotional development? (pp. 135 – 155). New York: Plenum Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Busby, J. & Suddendorf, T. (2005). Recalling yesterday and predicting tomorrow. Cognitive Development, 20, 362 – 372. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Caldarella, P. & Merrell, K. W. (1997). Common dimensions of social skills of children and adolescents: A taxonomy of positive behaviors. School Psychology Review, 26, 264 – 278. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Campos, J. J. , Frankel, C. B. & Camras, L. (2004). On the nature of emotion regulation. Child Development, 75, 377 – 394. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Carlson, S. M. (2010). Development of conscious control and imagination. In R. F. Baumeister, A. R. Mele & K. D. Vohs (Eds.), Free will and consciousness: How might they work? (pp. 135 – 152). New York: Oxford University Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Carlson, S. M. & Beck, D. M. (2009). Symbols as tools in the development of executive function. In A. Winsler, C. Fernyhough & I. Montero (Eds.), Private speech, executive functioning, and the development of verbal self-regulation (pp. 163 – 175). New York: Cambridge University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Carlson, S. M. & Wang, T. S. (2007). Inhibitory control and emotion regulation in preschool children. Cognitive Development, 22, 489 – 510. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Carlson, S. M. , Davis, A. C. & Leach, J. G. (2005). Less is more: Executive function and symbolic representation in preschool children. Psychological Science, 16, 609 – 616. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cole, P. M. (1986). Children’s spontaneous control of facial expression. Child Development, 57, 1309 – 1321. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cole, P. M. , Armstrong, L. M. & Pemberton, C. K. (2010). The role of language in the development of emotion regulation. In S. D. Calkins & M. A. Bell (Eds.), Child development at the intersection of emotion and cognition (pp. 59 – 77). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cole, P. M. , Dennis, T. A. , Smith-Simon, K. E. & Cohen, L. H. (2009). Preschoolers’ emotion regulation strategy understanding: Relations with emotion socialization and child self-regulation. Social Development, 18, 324 – 352. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cole, P. M. , Martin, S. E. & Dennis, T. A. (2004). Emotion regulation as a scientifi c construct: Methodological challenges and directions for child development research. Child Development, 75, 317 – 333. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Crick, N. R. & Dodge, K. A. (1994). A review and reformulation of social information-processing mechanisms in children’s social adjustment. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 74 – 101. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Davis, T. L. (1995). Gender differences in masking negative emotions: Ability or motivation? Developmental Psychology, 31, 660 – 667. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Denham, S. A. , Blair, K. A. , DeMulder, E. , Levitas, J. , Sawyer, K. , Auerbach-Major, S. T. , et al. (2003). Preschool emotional competence: Pathway to social competence? Child Development, 74, 238 – 256. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Denham, S. , Mason, T. , Caverly, S. , Schmidt, M. , Hackney, R. , Caswell, C. , et al. (2001). Preschoolers at play: Co-socialisers of emotional and social competence. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 25, 290 – 301. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Diener, M. L. & Mangelsdorf, S. C. (1999). Behavioral strategies for emotion regulation in toddlers: Associations with maternal involvement and emotional expressions. Infant Behavior & Development, 22, 569 – 583. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Elkonin, D. B. (2010). Psychologie des Spiels (hrsg. von B. Siebert und G. Rückriem). Berlin: Lehmanns Media. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Färber, M. & Holodynski, M. (2005, September). Echte oder vorgetäuschte Freude? Emotionale Ausdruckskontrolle bei 4-, 6- und 8-jährigen Kindern. Poster auf der 17. Tagung der Fachgruppe Entwicklungspsychologie, Bochum. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Fleer, M. & Hammer, M. (2013). Emotions in imaginative situations: The valued place of fairytales for supporting emotion regulation. Mind, Culture, and Activity. An International Journal, 20, 240 – 259. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Friedlmeier, W. (1999). Emotionsregulation in der Kindheit. In W. Friedlmeier & M. Holodynski (Hrsg.), Emotionale Entwicklung. Funktion, Regulation und soziokultureller Kontext von Emotionen (S. 197 – 218). Heidelberg: Spektrum Akademischer Verlag. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Friedman, S. L. & Scholnick, E. K. (Eds.). (1997). The developmental psychology of planning: Why, how, and when do we plan? Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Frijda, N. H. (1986). The emotions. New York: Cambridge University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Frijda, N. H. , Mesquita, B. , Sonnemans, J. & Van Goozen, S. (1991). The duration of affective phenomena or emotions, sentiments and passions. In K. T. Strongman (Ed.), International review of studies on emotion, Vol.1 (pp. 187 – 225). Chichester: Wiley. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Galyer, K. T. & Evans, I. M. (2001). Pretend play and the development of emotion regulation in preschool children. Early Child Development and Care, 166, 93 – 108. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Garner, P. W. & Power, T. G. (1996). Preschools’ emotional control in the disappointment paradigm and its relation to temperament, emotional knowledge, and family expressiveness. Child Development, 67, 1406 – 1419. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Giesbrecht, G. F. , Müller, U. & Miller, M. R. (2010). Psychological distancing in the development of executive function and emotion regulation. In B. W. Sokol, J. I. M. Carpendale , A. R. Young & G. Iarocci (Eds.), Self and social regulation: Social interaction and the development of social understanding and executive functions (pp. 337 – 357). New York: Oxford University Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gilliom, M. , Shaw, D. S. , Beck, J. E. , Schonberg, M. A. & Lukon, J. L. (2002). Anger regulation in disadvantaged preschool boys: Strategies, antecedents, and the development of self-control. Developmental Psychology, 38, 222 – 235. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gollwitzer, P. M. , Fujita, K. & Oettingen, G. (2004). Planning and the implementation of goals. In R. F. Baumeister & K. D. Vohs (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and applications (pp. 211 – 228). New York: Guilford. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Goschke, T. (2003). Voluntary action and cognitive control from a cognitive neuroscience perspective. In S. Maasen, W. Prinz & G. Roth (Eds.), Voluntary action. Brains, minds, and sociality (pp. 49 – 85). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Gross, J. J. & Levenson, R. W. (1997). Hiding feelings: The acute effects of inhibitung negative and positive emotion. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106, 95 – 103. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gross, J. J. & Thompson, R. A. (2007). Emotion regulation: Conceptual foundations. In J. J. Gross (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (pp. 3 – 24). New York: Guilford. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Gustafson, G. E. & Harris, K. L. (1990). Women’s responses to young infants’ cries. Developmental Psychology, 26, 144 – 152. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hampel, P. , Petermann, F. & Dickow, B. (2001). Stressverarbeitungsfragebogen für Kinder und Jugendliche. Göttingen: Hogrefe. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Harris, C. & Russel, J. A. (Eds.). (2013). Appraisal [Special Issue]. Emotion Review, 5 (2). First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Harter, S. & Whitesell, N. R. (1989). Developmental changes in children’s understanding of single, multiple, and blended emotion concepts. In C. Saarni & P. L. Harris (Eds.), Children’s understanding of emotion (pp. 81 – 116). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Hartmann, P. & Holodynski, M. (2008, July). Internalization of emotional expression in everyday situations from infancy to adulthood. Paper presented at the 20th Biennial Meeting of the International Society for the Study of Behavioural Development (ISSBD), University of Würzburg. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Holodynski, M. (unter Mitarbeit von W. Friedlmeier). (2006). Emotionen. Entwicklung und Regulation. Heidelberg: Springer. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Holodynski, M. , Seeger, D. , Hartmann, P. & Wörmann, V. (2012). Placing emotion regulation in a developmental framework of self-regulation. In K. C. Barrett, N. A. Fox , G. A. Morgan, D. Fidler & L. Daunhauer (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulatory processes in development: New directions and international perspectives (pp. 27 – 59). New York: Routledge. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Hongwanishkul, D. , Happaney, K. R. , Lee, W. S. C. & Zelazo, P. D. (2005). Assessment of hot and cool executive function in young children: Age-related changes and individual differences. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28, 617 – 644. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Izard, C. , Fine, S. , Schultz, D. , Mostow, A. , Ackerman, B. & Youngstrom, E. (2001). Emotion knowledge as a predictor of social behavior and academic competence in children at risk. Psychological Science, 12, 18 – 23. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Janke, B. (2010). Was kannst Du tun, um Dich nicht mehr zu fürchten? Strategien zur Furchtregulation im Kindergartenalter. Praxis der Kinderpsychologie und Kinderpsychiatrie, 59, 561 – 575. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kappas, A. (2008). Psssst! Dr. Jekyll and Mr. Hyde are actually the same person! A tale of regulation and emotion. In M. Vandekerckhove, C. von Schewe , S. Ismer, S. Jung & S. Kronast (Eds.), Regulating emotions. Culture, social necessity, and biological inheritance (pp. 15 – 38). Malden, MA: Blackwell. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kerr, A. & Zelazo, P. D. (2004). Development of „hot” executive function: The children’s gambling task. Brain and Cognition, 55, 148 – 157. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kieras, J. E. , Tobin, R. M. , Graziano, W. G. & Rothbart, M. K. (2005). You can’t always get what you want: Effortful control and children’s responses to undesirable gifts. Psychological Science, 16, 391 – 396. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kochanska, G. & Knaack, A. (2003). Effortful control as a personality characteristic of young children: Antecedents, correlates, and consequences. Journal of Personality, 71, 1087 – 1112. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kullik, A. & Petermann, F. (2012). Emotionsregulation im Kindesalter. Göttingen: Hogrefe. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Lagattuta, K. H. (2005). When you shouldn’t do what you want to do: Young children’s understanding of desires, rules, and emotions. Child Development, 76, 713 – 733. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Larsen, R. J. & Prizmic, Z. (2004). Affect regulation. In R. F. Baumeister & K. D. Vohs (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and applications (pp. 40 – 61). New York: Guilford. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Manfra, L. & Winsler, A. (2006). Preschool children’s awareness of private speech. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 30, 537 – 549. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Martin, J. & Failows, L. (2010). Executive function: Theoretical concerns. In B. W. Sokol, U. Müller , J. I. M. Carpendale, A. R. Young & G. Iarocci (Eds.), Self and social regulation: Social interaction and the development of social understanding and executive functions (pp. 35 – 55). New York: Oxford University Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McClelland, D. C. , Atkinson, J. W. , Clark, R. A. & Lowell, E. L. (1953). The achievement motive. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Meerum Terwogt, M. & Stegge, H. (1998). Children’s perspective on the emotional process. In A. Campbell & S. Muncer (Eds.), The social child (pp. 249 – 269). Hove: Psychology Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Mischel, W. & Ayduk, O. (2004). Willpower in a cognitive-affective processing system: The dynamics of delay of gratifi cation. In R. F. Baumeister & K. D. Vohs (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and applications (pp. 99 – 129). New York: Guilford. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Mischel, W. & Baker, N. (1975). Cognitive appraisals and transformations in delay behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31, 254 – 261. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Moore, B. , Mischel, W. & Zeiss, A. (1976). Comparative effects of the reward stimulus and its cognitive representation in voluntary delay. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34, 419 – 424. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Moors, A. & De Houwer, J. (2006). Automaticity: A theoretical and conceptual analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 297 – 326. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Moors, A. , Ellsworth, P. C. , Scherer, K. R. & Frijda, N. H. (2013). Appraisal theories of emotion: State of the art and future development. Emotion Review, 5, 119 – 124. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Morsella, E. (2009). The mechanisms of human action: Introduction and background. In E. Morsella, J. A. Bargh & P. M. Gollwitzer (Eds.), Oxford handbook of human action (pp. 1 – 32). New York: Oxford University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Morsella, E. , Bargh, J. A. & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2009). Oxford handbook of human action. New York: Oxford University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Müller, U. , Jacques, S. , Brocki, K. & Zelazo, P. D. (2009). The executive functions of language in preschool children. In A. Winsler, C. Fernyhough & I. Montero (Eds.), Private speech, executive functioning, and the development of verbal self-regulation (pp. 53 – 68). New York: Cambridge University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Müller, U. , Zelazo, P. D. , Hood, S. , Leone, T. & Rohrer, L. (2004). Interference control in a new rule use task: Age-related changes, labeling, and attention. Child Development, 75, 1594 – 1609. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Müller, U. , Zelazo, P. D. , Lurye, L. E. & Liebermann, D. P. (2008). The effect of labeling on preschool children’s performance in the Dimensional Change Card Sort Task. Cognitive Development, 23, 395 – 408. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Neal, D. T. & Wood, W. (2009). Automaticity in situ and in the lab: The nature of habit in daily life. In E. Morsella, J. A. Bargh & P. M. Gollwitzer (Eds.), Oxford handbook of human action (pp. 442 – 456). New York: Oxford University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Petermann, F. & Kullik, A. (2011). Frühe Emotionsdysregulation: Ein Indikator für psychische Störungen im Kindesalter. Kindheit und Entwicklung, 20, 186 – 196. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Petermann, F. & Wiedebusch, S. (2008). Emotionale Kompetenz bei Kindern (2., erweiterte Aufl.). Göttingen: Hogrefe. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Pons, F. , Harris, P. L. & de Rosnay, M. (2004). Emotion comprehension between 3 and 11 years: Developmental periods and hierarchical organization. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 1, 127 – 152. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rottleuthner-Lutter, M. (1987). Verwenden Kindergarten- und Grundschulkinder die Unterscheidung zwischen Ausdruck und Erleben, wenn sie über Gefühle reden? Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 19, 287 – 299. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Saarni, C. (1984). An observational study of children’s attempts to monitor their expressive behavior. Child Development, 55, 1504 – 1513. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Saarni, C. (1999). The development of emotional competence. New York: Guilford. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Saarni, C. & Weber, H. (1999). Emotional displays and dissemblance in childhood: Implications for self-presentation. In P. Philippot, R. S. Feldman & E. J. Coats (Eds.), The social context of nonverbal behavior (pp. 71 – 105). New York: Cambridge University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Sánchez, T. , Montero, I. & Méndez, C. (2006). Private speech and strategies for emotional self-regulation in preschool-aged children. In I. Montero (Ed.), Current research trends in private speech. Madrid: Publicaciones de la Universidad Autónoma de Madrid. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Schmidt-Atzert, L. (2008). Klassifikation von Emotionen. In W. Janke, M. Schmidt-Daffy & G. Debus (Hrsg.), Experimentelle Emotionspsychologie. Methodische Ansätze, Probleme, Ergebnisse (S. 179 – 191). Lengerich: Pabst. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Selman, R. L. (1981). What children understand of the intrapsychic processes. In E. K. Shapiro & E. Weber (Eds.), Cognitive and affective growth (pp. 187 – 215). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Shoda, Y. , Mischel, W. & Peake, P. K. (1990). Predicting adolescent cognitive and self-regulatory competencies from preschool delay of gratification. Developmental Psychology, 26, 978 – 986. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Singer, D. G. & Singer, J. L. (1990). The house of make-believe: Children’s play and the developing imagination. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Sroufe, L. A. (1996). Emotional development: The organization of emotional life in the early years. New York: Cambridge University Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Stegge, H. & Meerum Terwogt, M. (2007). Awareness and regulation of emotion in typical and atypical development. In J. J. Gross (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (pp. 269 – 286). New York: Guilford. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Suddendorf, T. & Corballis, M. C. (2007). The evolution of foresight: What is mental time travel, and is it unique to humans? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30, 299 – 313. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Thompson, R. A. (1990). Emotion and self-regulation. In R. A. Thompson (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (Vol. 36, pp. 367 – 467). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Thompson, R. A. (1994). Emotion regulation: A theme in search of definition. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59 (2/3), 25 – 52. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Thompson, R. A. & Meyer, S. (2007). Socialization of emotion regulation in the family. In J. J. Gross (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (pp. 249 – 268). New York: Guilford. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Tobin, R. M. & Graziano, W. G. (2010). Delay of gratification: A review of fifty years of regulation research. In R. H. Hoyle (Ed.), Handbook of personality and self-regulation (pp. 47 – 63). Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Vygotskij, L. S. (1934/2002). Denken und Sprechen (hrsg. von G. Rückriem und J. Lompscher). Weinheim: Beltz. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1933/2010). Das Spiel und seine Bedeutung in der psychischen Entwicklung des Kindes. In D. B. Elkonin (Hrsg.), Psychologie des Spiels. Berlin: Lehmanns Media. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Vygotsky, L. S. & Luria, A. R. (1994). Tool and symbol in child development. In R. v. d. Veer & J. Valsiner (Eds.), The Vygotsky reader (pp. 99 – 174). Oxford: Blackwell. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Winsler, A. (2009). Still talking to ourselves after all these years: A review of current research on private speech. In A. Winsler, C. Fernyhough & I. Montero (Eds.), Private speech, executive functioning, and the development of verbal self-regulation (pp. 3 – 41). New York: Cambridge University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Winsler, A. , Fernyhough, C. & Montero, I. (2009). Private speech, executive functioning, and the development of verbal self-regulation. New York: Cambridge University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Wranik, T. , Feldman Barrett, L. & Salovey, P. (2007). Intelligent emotion regulation: Is knowledge power? In J. J. Gross (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (pp. 393 – 407). New York: Guilford. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Zelazo, P. D. & Carlson, S. M. (2012). Hot and cool executive function in childhood and adolescence: Development and plasticity. Child Development Perspectives, 6, 354 – 360. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Zelazo, P. D. & Cunningham, W. A. (2007). Executive function: Mechanisms underlying emotion regulation. In J. J. Gross (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (pp. 135 – 158). New York: Guilford. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Zelazo, P. D. , Qu, L. & Kesek, A. C. (2010). Hot executive function: Emotion and the development of cognitive control. In S. D. Calkins & M. A. Bell (Eds.), Child development at the intersection of emotion and cognition (pp. 97 – 111). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Zelazo, P. D. , Qu, L. & Müller, U. (2005). Hot and cool aspects of executive function: Relations in early development. In W. Schneider, R. Schumann-Hengsteler & B. Sodian (Eds.), Young children’s cognitive development: Interrelationships among executive functioning, working memory, verbal ability, and theory of mind (pp. 71 – 93). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar