Skip to main content
Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1026/0033-3042.55.3.118

Zusammenfassung. Der Implizite Assoziationstest (IAT) ist wahrscheinlich das bekannteste und am weitesten verbreitete Verfahren zur Messung automatisch aktivierter Assoziationen. Trotz der vorliegenden Evidenz für dessen Validität gibt es allerdings nach wie vor Kritik am IAT, die vor allem auf der ungeklärten Frage nach den zugrundeliegenden psychologischen Prozessen beruht. Der folgende Artikel gibt einen Überblick über die vorhandene Forschung zum IAT, um auf diese Weise sowohl die Reichweite als auch die Grenzen des Verfahrens aufzuzeigen. Thematisiert werden dabei Zusammenhänge zu expliziten Fragebogenmaßen, Ergebnisse zur Vorhersagekraft des IAT, Studien zu Kontext- und Materialeffekten, sowie die Rolle systematischer Fehlervarianz. Als Resultat dieses Überblicks stehen eine Reihe von allgemeinen Schlussfolgerungen zur konvergenten, diskriminanten, prädiktiven, inkrementellen und internen Validität des IAT, sowie einige kritische Anmerkungen zu weit verbreiteten Interpretationen in der IAT-Forschung.


The Implicit Association Test as a measure of automatically activated associations: Range and limits

Abstract. The Implicit Association Test (IAT) is probably the most well known and most widespread measure of automatically activated associations. Even though there is supportive evidence for the validity of the IAT, there are still controversies about its underlying psychological processes. The following article provides an overview of the available research on the IAT, illustrating both the range and the limits of the measure. This review includes the relation to explicit self-report measures, evidence for its predictive ability, studies on context and material effects, and the role of systematic error variance. Based on this review, we draw conclusions about the convergent, discriminant, predictive, incremental, and internal validity of the IAT, and critically discuss some common interpretations in IAT research.

Literatur

  • Allport, D. A. , Styles, E. A. , Hsieh, S. (1994). Shifting intentional set: Exploring the dynamic control of tasks. In C. Umilta & M. Moscovitch (Eds.), Attention and performance XV: Conscious and nonconscious information processing (pp. 421-452). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Asendorpf, J. B. , Banse, R. , Mücke, D. (2002). Double dissociation between explicit and implicit personality self-concept: The case of shy behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83 , 380– 393 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ashburn-Nardo, L. , Voils, C. I. , Monteith, M. J. (2001). Implicit associations as seeds of intergroup bias: How easily do they take root. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81 , 789– 799 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Banse, R. (1999). Automatic evaluation of self and significant others: Affective priming in close relationships. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 16 , 803– 821 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Banse, R. (2001). Affective priming with liked and disliked persons: Prime visibility determines congruency and incongruency effects. Cognition and Emotion, 15 , 501– 520 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Banse, R. , Fischer, I. (2002, Juli). Implicit and explicit aggressiveness and the prediction of aggressive behavior . Poster bei der 11th European Conference on Personality by the European Society for Personality Psychology, Jena First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Banse, R. , Gawronski, B. (2003). Die Skala Motivation zu vorurteilsfreiem Verhalten: Skaleneigenschaften und Validierung. Diagnostica, 49 , 4– 13 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Banse, R. , Seise, J. , Zerbes, N. (2001). Implicit attitudes towards homosexuality: Reliability, validity, and controllability of the IAT. Zeitschrift für Experimentelle Psychologie, 48 , 145– 160 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Blair, I. V. , Ma, J. , Lenton, A. (2001). Imagining stereotypes away: The moderation of implicit stereotypes through mental imagery. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81 , 828– 841 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Blümke, M. , Friese, M. (2004). Do irrelevant features of stimuli influence IAT effects? . Manuscript submitted for publication First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Bosson, J. K. , Swann, W. B. , Pennebaker, J. W. (2000). Stalking the perfect measure of implicit self-esteem: The blind men and the elephant revisited?. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79 , 631– 643 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Brendl, C. M. , Markman, A. B. , Messner, C. (2001). How do indirect measures of evaluation work? Evaluating the inference of prejudice in the Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81 , 760– 773 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Campbell, D. T. , Fiske, D. W. (1959). Convergent and discriminant validation by the multitrait-multimethod matrix. Psychological Bulletin, 56 , 81– 105 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Chee, M. L. , Sriram, N. , Soon, C. S. , Lee, K. M. (2000). Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and the implicit association of concepts and attributes. Neuroreport, 11 , 135– 140 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Conrey, F. R. , Sherman, J. W. , Gawronski, B. , Hugenberg, K. , Groom, C. (2004). Beyond automaticity and control: The Quad-Model of behavioral response . Manuscript submitted for publication First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Cunningham, W. A. , Preacher, K. J. , Banaji, M. R. (2001). Implicit attitude measurement: Consistency, stability, and covergent validity. Psychological Science, 12 , 163– 170 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cunningham, W. A. , Nezlek, J. B. , Banaji, M. R. (in press) Implicit and explicit ethnocentrism: Revisiting the ideologies of prejudice. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Dasgupta, N. , Greenwald, A. G. (2001). On the malleability of automatic attitudes: Combating automatic prejudice with images of admired and disliked individuals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81 , 800– 814 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • De Houwer, J. (2001). A structural and process analysis of the Implicit Association Test. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 37 , 443– 451 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • De Houwer, J. (2003). A structural analysis of indirect measures of attitudes. In J. Musch & K. C. Klauer (Eds.), The psychology of evaluation: Affective processes in cognition and emotion (pp. 219-244). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Devine, P. G. (2001). Implicit prejudice and stereotyping: How automatic are they?. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81 , 757– 759 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Duncan, B. L. (1976). Differential perception and attribution of intergroup violence: Testing the lower limits of stereotyping of Blacks. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34 , 590– 598 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dunning, D. , Sherman, D. A. (1997). Stereotypes and tacit inference. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73 , 459– 471 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Egloff, B. , Schmukle, S. C. (2002). Predictive validity of an Implicit Association Test for assessing anxiety. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83 , 1441– 1455 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fazio, R. H. , Olson, M. A. (2003). Implicit measures in social cognition research: Their meaning and use. Annual Review of Psychology, 54 , 297– 327 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fazio, R. H. , Towles-Schwen, T. (1999). The MODE model of attitude-behavior processes. In S. Chaiken & Y. Trope (Eds.), Dual process theories in social psychology (pp. 97-116). New York: Guilford Press First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Festinger, L. , Carlsmith, J. M. (1959). Cognitive consequences of forced compliance. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 58 , 203– 210 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Florack, A. , Scarabis, M. , Bless, H. (2001a). Der Einfluss wahrgenommener Bedrohung auf die Nutzung automatischer Assoziationen bei der Personenbeurteilung. Zeitschrift für Sozialpsychologie, 32 , 249– 260 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Florack, A. , Scarabis, M. , Bless, H. (2001b). When do associations matter? The use of automatic associations towards ethnic groups in person judgments. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 37 , 518– 524 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Foroni, F. , Mayr, U. (in press) The power of a story: New, automatic associations from a single reading of a short scenario. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Gawronski, B. (2000). Falsifikationismus und Holismus in der experimentellen Psychologie: Logische Grundlagen und methodologische Konsequenzen. Zeitschrift für Sozialpsychologie, 31 , 3– 17 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Gawronski, B. (2002). What does the Implicit Association Test measure? A test of the convergent and discriminant validity of prejudice related IATs. Experimental Psychology, 49 , 171– 180 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Gawronski, B. , Bodenhausen, G. V. (2004). Accessibility effects on implicit social cognition: The role of accessible content versus accessibility experiences . Manuscript submitted for publication First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Gawronski, B. , Ehrenberg, K. , Banse, R. , Zukova, J. , Klauer, K. C. (2003a). It’s in the mind of the beholder: The impact of stereotypic associations on category-based and individuating impression formation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39 , 16– 30 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gawronski, B. , Geschke, D. , Banse, R. (2003b). Implicit bias in impression formation: Associations influence the construal of individuating information. European Journal of Social Psychology, 33 , 573– 589 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gawronski, B. , Strack, F. (2004). On the propositional nature of cognitive consistency: Dissonance changes explicit, but not implicit attitudes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40 , 535– 542 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Govan, C. L. , Williams, K. D. (2004). Changing the affective valence of the stimulus items influences the IAT by redefining the category labels. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40 , 357– 365 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gray, N. S. , McCulloch, M. J , Smith, J. , Morris, M. , Snowden, R. J. (2003). Violence viewed by psychopathic murderers: Adapting a revealing test may expose those psychopaths who are most likely to kill. Nature, 423 , 497– 498 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Greenwald, A. G. , Banaji, M. R. (1995). Implicit social cognition: Attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychological Review, 102 , 4– 27 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Greenwald, A. G. , McGhee, D. E. , Schwartz, J. K. L. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74 , 1464– 1480 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Greenwald, A. G. , Nosek, B. A. (2003). Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: I. An improved scoring algorithm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85 , 197– 216 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hofmann, W. , Gawronski, B. , Gschwendner, T. , Le, H. , Schmitt, M. (2003). A meta-analysis on the correlation between the Implicit Association Test and explicit self-report measures . Manuscript submitted for publication First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Hofmann, W. , Gschwendner, T. , Schmitt, M. (2003). On the moderation of the implicit-explicit consistency: The role of introspection and adjustment . Manuscript submitted for publication First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Hugenberg, K. , Bodenhausen, G. V. (2003). Facing prejudice: Implicit prejudice and the perception of facial threat. Psychological Science, 14 , 640– 643 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hugenberg, K. , Bodenhausen, G. V. (2004). Ambiguity in social categorization: The role of prejudice and facial affect in racial categorization. Psychological Science, 15 , 342– 345 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hummert, M. L. , Gartska, T. A. , O’Brien, L. T. , Greenwald, A. G. , Mellott, D. S. (2002). Using the Implicit Association Test to measure age differences in implicit social cognitions. Psychology & Aging, 17 , 482– 495 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Johnston, W. A. , Hawley, K. J. , Plewe, S. H. , Elliott, J. M. G. , DeWitt, M. J. (1990). Attention capture by novel stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 119 , 397– 411 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Karpinski, A. , Hilton, J. L. (2001). Attitudes and the Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81 , 774– 788 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kim, D. (2003). Voluntary controllability of the Implicit Association Test (IAT). Social Psychology Quarterly, 66 , 83– 96 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Klauer, K. C. , Wegener, I. (1998). Unraveling social categorization in the “Who said what?” paradigm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75 , 1155– 1178 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kray, J. , Lindenberger, U. (2000). Adult age differences in task switching. Psychology & Aging, 15 , 126– 147 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kühnen, U. , Schießl, M. , Bauer, N. , Paulig, N. , Pöhlmann, C. , Schmidthals, K. (2001). How robust is the IAT? Measuring and manipulating implicit attitudes of East- and West- Germans. Zeitschrift für Experimentelle Psychologie, 48 , 135– 144 First citation in articleAbstractGoogle Scholar

  • Kunda, Z. , Sherman-Williams, B. (1993). Stereotypes and the construal of individuating information. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 19 , 90– 99 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Lindsay, D. S. , Jacoby, L. L. (1994). Stroop process dissociations: The relationship between facilitation and interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 20 , 219– 234 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lowery, B. S. , Hardin, C. D. , Sinclair, S. (2001). Social influence effects on automatic racial prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81 , 842– 855 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Maison, D. , Greenwald, A. G. , Bruin, R. (2001). The Implicit Association Test as a measure of implicit consumer attitudes. Polish Psychological Bulletin, 32 , 1– 9 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • McConnell, A. R. , Leibold, J. M. (2001). Relations among the Implicit Association Test, discriminatory behavior, and explicit measures of racial attitudes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 37 , 435– 442 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McFarland, S. G. , Crouch, Z. (2002). A cognitive skill confound on the Implicit Association Test. Social Cognition, 20 , 483– 510 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mierke, J. , Klauer, K. C. (2001). Implicit association measurement with the IAT: Evidence for effects of executive control processes. Zeitschrift für Experimentelle Psychologie, 48 , 107– 122 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Mierke, J. , Klauer, K. C. (2003). Method-specific variance in the Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85 , 1180– 1192 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mitchell, J. P. , Nosek, B. A. , Banaji, M. R. (2003). Contextual variations in implicit evaluation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132 , 455– 469 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Nosek, B. A. (2004). Moderators of the relationship between explicit and implicit attitudes . Manuscript submitted for publication First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Perugini, M. (in press) Predictive models of implicit and explicit attitudes. British Journal of Social Psychology, First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Phelps, E. A. , O’Connor, K. J. , Cunningham, W. A. , Funayama, E. S. , Gatenby, J. C. , Gore, J. C. , Banaji, M. R. (2000). Performance on indirect measures of race evaluation predicts amygdala activation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12 , 729– 738 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Plessner, H. , Banse, R. (2001). Attitude measurement using the Implicit Association Test. Zeitschrift für Experimentelle Psychologie, 48 , 82– 84 First citation in articleAbstractGoogle Scholar

  • Pratto, F. , John, O. P. (1991). Automatic vigilance: The attention-grabbing power of negative social information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61 , 380– 391 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Richeson, J. A. , Ambady, N. (2001). Who’s in charge? Effects of situational roles on automatic gender bias. Sex Roles, 44 , 493– 512 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Richeson, J. A. , Ambady, N. (2003). Effects of situational power on automatic racial prejudice. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39 , 177– 183 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rogers, R. D. , Monsell, S. (1995). Costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124 , 207– 231 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rothermund, K. , Wentura, D. (2001). Figure-ground asymmetries in the Implicit Association Test. Zeitschrift für Experimentelle Psychologie, 48 , 94– 106 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Rothermund, K. , Wentura, D. (2004). Underlying processes in the Implicit Association Test (IAT): Dissociating salience from associations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133 , 139– 165 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rudman, L. A. , Feinberg, J. , Fairchild, K. (2002). Minority members’ implicit attitudes: Automatic ingroup bias as a function of group status. Social Cognition, 20 , 294– 320 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rudman, L. A. , Glick, P. (2001). Prescriptive gender stereotypes and backlash toward agentic women. Journal of Social Issues, 57 , 743– 762 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rudman, L. A. , Greenwald, A. G. , Mellott, D. S. , Schwartz, J. L. K. (1999). Measuring the automatic components of prejudice: Flexibility and generality of the Implicit Association Test. Social Cognition, 17 , 437– 465 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sagar, H. A. , Schofield, J. W. (1980). Racial and behavioral cues in black and white children’s perceptions of ambiguously aggressive acts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39 , 590– 598 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schmukle, S. C. , Egloff, B. (in press) Does the Implicit Association Test for assessing anxiety measure state and trait variance?. European Journal of Personality, First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Skowronski, J. J. , Lawrence, M. A. (2001). A comparative study of the implicit and explicit gender attitudes of children and college students. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 25 , 155– 165 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Steffens, M. C. (in press) Is the Implicit Association Test immune to faking?. Experimental Psychology, First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Steffens, M. C. , Buchner, A. (2003). Implicit Association Test: Separating transsituationally stable and variable components of attitudes toward gay men. Experimental Psychology, 50 , 33– 48 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Steffens, M. C. , Plewe, I. (2001). Items’ cross-category associations as a confounding factor in the Implicit Association Test. Zeitschrift für Experimentelle Psychologie, 48 , 123– 134 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Taylor, S. E. , Fiske, S. T. , Etcoff, N. L. , Ruderman, A. J. (1978). Categorical and contextual bases of person memory and stereotyping. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36 , 778– 793 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Teachman, B. A. , Gregg, A. P. , Woody, S. R. (2001). Implicit associations for fear-relevant stimuli among individuals with snake and spider fears. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110 , 226– 235 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Teige, S. , Schnabel, K. , Banse, R. , Asendorpf, J. B. (in press) Assessment of multiple implicit self-concept dimensions using the Extrinsic Affective Simon Task (EAST). European Journal of Personality, First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Townsend, J. T. , Ashby, F. G. (1983). The stochastic modeling of elementary psychological processes . New York. Cambridge University Press First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Wiers, R. W. , Van Woerden, N. , Smulders, F. T. Y. , De Jong, P. J. (2002). Implicit and explicit alcohol related cognitions in heavy and light drinkers. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111 , 648– 658 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wilson, T. D. , Lindsey, S. , Schooler, T. Y. (2000). A model of dual attitudes. Psychological Review, 107 , 101– 126 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wittenbrink, B. , Judd, C. M. , Park, B. (2001). Spontaneous prejudice in context: Variability in automatically activated attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81 , 815– 827 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar