Skip to main content
Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1026//0949-3946.48.2.107

Abstract. It is argued that a model of goal-independent spreading activation in a social or semantic knowledge structure is insufficient to explain implicit association effects in the IAT (Greenwald, McGhee, & Schwartz, 1998). An alternative account is proposed, which attributes IAT effects to differential costs for switching between task sets. Two experiments were conduced to test this account. In Experiment 1, specific task-set switching cost was a function of IAT condition: switching between tasks was associated with significantly more cost in the incompatible IAT phase. In a second experiment the magnitude of the IAT effect was reduced when task-set reconfiguration was possible in advance of or simultaneously with the upcoming stimulus. The results are discussed with respect to recently suggested accounts of the effect.


“Implizite Assoziationsmessung mit dem IAT: Evidenz für Effekte exekutiver Kontrollprozesse

Zusammenfassung. Es wird argumentiert, dass ein Modell zielunabhängiger Aktivierungsausbreitung in einem sozialen oder semantischen Netzwerk nicht ausreicht, um Assoziationseffekte im IAT (Greenwald, McGhee, & Schwartz, 1998) zu erklären. Ein alternativer Ansatz wird vorgeschlagen, der IAT Effekte auf bedingungsanhängige Aufgabenwechselkosten zurückführt. Zwei Experimente prüfen diesen Ansatz. Im ersten Experiment zeigte sich, dass spezifische Aufgabenwechselkosten eine Funktion der IAT-Bedingung sind: Aufgabenwechsel waren in der inkompatiblen IAT-Bedingung mit bedeutsam höheren Kosten verbunden. In einem zweiten Experiment war das Ausmaó des IAT-Effekts reduziert, wenn die Möglichkeit zu vorbereitender oder simultaner Einstellung auf den Aufgabenwechsel gegeben wurde. Die Ergebnisse werden im Hinblick auf aktuelle Erklärungsmodelle des Effekts diskutiert.

References

  • Allport, D. A., Styles, E. A., Hsieh, S.(1994). Shifting intentional set: Exploring the dynamic control of tasks.. In C. Umilta & M. Moscovitch (Eds.), Attention and performance XV, (pp. 421– 452 Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Banse, R., Seise, J., Zerbes, N.(2001). Implicit attitudes towards homosexuality: Reliability, validity, and controllability of the IAT.. Zeitschrift für Experimentelle Psychologie, 48, (this issue) 145– 160 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Bargh, J. A., Chaiken, S., Raymond, P., Hymes, C.(1996). The automatic evaluation effect: Unconditional automatic attitude activation with a pronunciation task.. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 32, 104– 128 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Brendl, C. M., Markman, A. B., Messner, C.(in press). How do indirect measures of evaluation work? Evaluating the interference of prejudice in the Implicit Association Test.. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology., First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Collins, A. M., Loftus, E. F.(1975). A spreading-activation theory of semantic priming processing.. Psychological Review, 82, 407– 428 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Collins, A. M., Quillian, M. R.(1969). Retrieval time from semantic memory.. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 2, 240– 248 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Celex(1995). The Celex Lexical Database, Release 2.. Nijmegen: Center for Lexical Information.. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Dasgupta, N., McGhee, D. E., Greenwald, A. G., Banaji, M. R.(2000). Automatic preference for White Americans: Eliminating the familiarity explanation.. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 36, 316– 328 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • De Houwer, J.(in press). A structural and process analysis of the Implicit Association Test.. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Ehrenberg, K., Klauer, K. C., Wegener, I.(2000). Hierarchische Verarbeitung multipler sozialer Kategorieinformation: Die Dominanz von Geschlechtskategorien . [Hierarchical processing of multiple social category information: The dominance of gender categories]. Paper presented at the 42. Tagung experimentell arbeitender Psychologen, Braunschweig.. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Gopher, D.(1996). Attention control: explorations of the work of an executive controller.. Cognitive Brain Research, 5, 23– 38 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Greenwald, A. G., Banaji, M. R., Rudman, L. A., Farnham, S. D., Nosek, B. A., Rosier, M.(2000). Prologue toa unified theory of attitudes, stereotypes, and self-concept.. In J. P. Forgas (Ed.), Feeling and thinking:The role of affect in social cognition and behavior (pp. 308– 330 ). New York: Cambridge University Press.. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., Schwartz, J. L. J.(1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The implicit association test.. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1464– 1480 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Klauer, K. C., Musch, J.(in press). Does sunshine prime loyal? Affective priming in the naming task.. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Klauer, K. C., Wegener, I.(1998). Unraveling social categorization in the “Who Said What?\ paradigm.. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 1155– 1178 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Kornblum, S.(1992). Dimensional overlap and dimensional relevance in stimulus-response and stimulus-stimulus compatibility.. In G. E. Stelmach & J. Requin (Eds.), Tutorials in motor behavior, Volume 2 (pp. 743– 777 ). Amsterdam: North Holland.. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Kramer, A. F., Hahn, S., Gopher, D.(1999). Task coordination and aging: explorations of executive control processes in the task-switching paradigm.. Acta Psychologica, 101, 339– 378 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Kray, J., Lindenberger, U.(2000). Adult age differences in task switching.. Psychology and Aging, 15, 126– 147 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Kühnen, U., Schießl, U., Bauer, N., Paulig, N., Pöhlmann, N., Schmidthals, N.(2001). How robust is the IAT? Measuring and manipulating implicit attitudes of East- and West-Germans.. Zeitschrift für Experimentelle Psychologie, 48, (this issue), 135– 144 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Los, S. A.(1996). On the origin of mixing costs: Exploring information processing in pure and mixed blocks of trials.. Acta Psychologica, 94, 145– 188 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • MacLeod, C. M.(1991). Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: An integrative review.. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 163– 203 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Meiran, N.(1996). Reconfiguration of processing mode prior to task performance.. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 1423– 1442 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Monsell, S., Yeung, N., Azuma, R.(2000). Reconfiguration of task-set: Is it easier to switch to the weaker task?. Psychological Research, 63, 250– 264 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Neely, J.(1991). Semantic priming effects in visual word recognition: A selective review of current findingsand theories.. In D. Besner & G. Humphreys (Eds.), Basic processes in reading: Visual word recognition (pp. 264– 337 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Neumann, O.(1996). Theories of attention.. In O. Neumann & A. F. Sanders (Eds.), Handbook of perception and action. Volume 3: Attention (pp. 389– 446 ). San Diego: Academic Press.. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Neumann, R., Ebert, M., Gabel, B., Guelsdorff, J., Krannich, H., Lauterbach, C., Wiedl, K.(1998). Vorurteile zwischen Bayern und Norddeutschen: Die Anwendung einer neuen Methode zur Erfassung evaluativer Assoziationen in Vorurteilen [Prejudice prevailing between Bavarians and North Germans: Application of a new method for the assessment of evaluative associations in prejudices].. Zeitschrift für Experimentelle Psychologie, 45, 99– 108 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Neumann, R., Totzke, I., Popp, C., Fernandez, O.(2000). Distinct attitudes despite similar targets: Comparing German students' attitudes towards “blacks\ and foreigners.. Unpublished manuscript.. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Norman, D., Shallice, T.(1986). Attention to action: Willed and automatic control of behavior.. In R. J. Davidson, G. E. Schwartz, & D. Shapiro (Eds.), Consciousness and self regulation, Volume 4 (pp. 1– 18 ). New York: Plenum Press.. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pashler, H., & Baylis, G.(1991). Procedural learning: 2. Intertrial repetition effects in speeded-choice tasks.. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 33– 48 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rogers, R. D., Monsell, S.(1995). Costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks.. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 207– 231 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rothermund, K., & Wentura, D.(2001). Figure-ground-asymmetries in the Implicit Association Test (IAT).. Zeitschrift für Experimentelle Psychologie, 48, (this issue), 94– 106 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Schwibbe, M., Räder, K., Schwibbe, G., Borchardt, M., Geiken-Pophanken, G.(1994). Zum emotionalen Gehalt von Substantiven, Adjektiven und Verben (On the emotional content of nouns, adjectives, and verbs).. In W. Hager & M. Hasselhorn (Eds.), Handbuch deutschsprachiger Wortnormen (pp. 272– 284 ). Göttingen: Hogrefe.. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Simon, J. R.(1990). The effect of an irrelevant directional cue on human information processing.. In R. W. Proctor & T. G. Reeve (Eds.), Stimulus-response compatibility: An integrated perspective. Advances in psychology, 65, (pp. 31– 86 ). Amsterdam: North Holland.. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Stangor, C., Lynch, L., Duan, C., Glass, B.(1992). Categorization of individuals on the basis of multiple social features.. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 207– 218 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Steffens, M. C., Plewe, I.(2001). Items' cross-category associataions as a confounding factor in the Implicit Association Test.. Zeitschrift für Experimentelle Psychologie, 48, (this issue), 123– 134 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Stroop, J. R.(1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions.. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643– 662 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tipper, S. P.(1985). The negative priming effect: Inhibitory effects of ignored primes.. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 37, 571– 590 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tipper, S. P., MacQueen, G. M., Brehaut, J. C.(1988). Negative priming between response modalities: Evidence for the central locus of inhibition in selective attention.. Perception and Psychophysics, 43, 45– 52 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Wylie, G., Allport, A.(2000). Task switching and the measurement of “switch costs\.. Psychological Research, 18, 212– 233 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar